• Search Menu

Sign in through your institution

  • Browse content in Arts and Humanities
  • Browse content in Archaeology
  • Anglo-Saxon and Medieval Archaeology
  • Archaeological Methodology and Techniques
  • Archaeology by Region
  • Archaeology of Religion
  • Archaeology of Trade and Exchange
  • Biblical Archaeology
  • Contemporary and Public Archaeology
  • Environmental Archaeology
  • Historical Archaeology
  • History and Theory of Archaeology
  • Industrial Archaeology
  • Landscape Archaeology
  • Mortuary Archaeology
  • Prehistoric Archaeology
  • Underwater Archaeology
  • Zooarchaeology
  • Browse content in Architecture
  • Architectural Structure and Design
  • History of Architecture
  • Residential and Domestic Buildings
  • Theory of Architecture
  • Browse content in Art
  • Art Subjects and Themes
  • History of Art
  • Industrial and Commercial Art
  • Theory of Art
  • Biographical Studies
  • Byzantine Studies
  • Browse content in Classical Studies
  • Classical History
  • Classical Philosophy
  • Classical Mythology
  • Classical Numismatics
  • Classical Literature
  • Classical Reception
  • Classical Art and Architecture
  • Classical Oratory and Rhetoric
  • Greek and Roman Papyrology
  • Greek and Roman Epigraphy
  • Greek and Roman Law
  • Greek and Roman Archaeology
  • Late Antiquity
  • Religion in the Ancient World
  • Social History
  • Digital Humanities
  • Browse content in History
  • Colonialism and Imperialism
  • Diplomatic History
  • Environmental History
  • Genealogy, Heraldry, Names, and Honours
  • Genocide and Ethnic Cleansing
  • Historical Geography
  • History by Period
  • History of Emotions
  • History of Agriculture
  • History of Education
  • History of Gender and Sexuality
  • Industrial History
  • Intellectual History
  • International History
  • Labour History
  • Legal and Constitutional History
  • Local and Family History
  • Maritime History
  • Military History
  • National Liberation and Post-Colonialism
  • Oral History
  • Political History
  • Public History
  • Regional and National History
  • Revolutions and Rebellions
  • Slavery and Abolition of Slavery
  • Social and Cultural History
  • Theory, Methods, and Historiography
  • Urban History
  • World History
  • Browse content in Language Teaching and Learning
  • Language Learning (Specific Skills)
  • Language Teaching Theory and Methods
  • Browse content in Linguistics
  • Applied Linguistics
  • Cognitive Linguistics
  • Computational Linguistics
  • Forensic Linguistics
  • Grammar, Syntax and Morphology
  • Historical and Diachronic Linguistics
  • History of English
  • Language Evolution
  • Language Reference
  • Language Acquisition
  • Language Variation
  • Language Families
  • Lexicography
  • Linguistic Anthropology
  • Linguistic Theories
  • Linguistic Typology
  • Phonetics and Phonology
  • Psycholinguistics
  • Sociolinguistics
  • Translation and Interpretation
  • Writing Systems
  • Browse content in Literature
  • Bibliography
  • Children's Literature Studies
  • Literary Studies (Romanticism)
  • Literary Studies (American)
  • Literary Studies (Asian)
  • Literary Studies (European)
  • Literary Studies (Eco-criticism)
  • Literary Studies (Modernism)
  • Literary Studies - World
  • Literary Studies (1500 to 1800)
  • Literary Studies (19th Century)
  • Literary Studies (20th Century onwards)
  • Literary Studies (African American Literature)
  • Literary Studies (British and Irish)
  • Literary Studies (Early and Medieval)
  • Literary Studies (Fiction, Novelists, and Prose Writers)
  • Literary Studies (Gender Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Graphic Novels)
  • Literary Studies (History of the Book)
  • Literary Studies (Plays and Playwrights)
  • Literary Studies (Poetry and Poets)
  • Literary Studies (Postcolonial Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Queer Studies)
  • Literary Studies (Science Fiction)
  • Literary Studies (Travel Literature)
  • Literary Studies (War Literature)
  • Literary Studies (Women's Writing)
  • Literary Theory and Cultural Studies
  • Mythology and Folklore
  • Shakespeare Studies and Criticism
  • Browse content in Media Studies
  • Browse content in Music
  • Applied Music
  • Dance and Music
  • Ethics in Music
  • Ethnomusicology
  • Gender and Sexuality in Music
  • Medicine and Music
  • Music Cultures
  • Music and Media
  • Music and Religion
  • Music and Culture
  • Music Education and Pedagogy
  • Music Theory and Analysis
  • Musical Scores, Lyrics, and Libretti
  • Musical Structures, Styles, and Techniques
  • Musicology and Music History
  • Performance Practice and Studies
  • Race and Ethnicity in Music
  • Sound Studies
  • Browse content in Performing Arts
  • Browse content in Philosophy
  • Aesthetics and Philosophy of Art
  • Epistemology
  • Feminist Philosophy
  • History of Western Philosophy
  • Metaphysics
  • Moral Philosophy
  • Non-Western Philosophy
  • Philosophy of Language
  • Philosophy of Mind
  • Philosophy of Perception
  • Philosophy of Science
  • Philosophy of Action
  • Philosophy of Law
  • Philosophy of Religion
  • Philosophy of Mathematics and Logic
  • Practical Ethics
  • Social and Political Philosophy
  • Browse content in Religion
  • Biblical Studies
  • Christianity
  • East Asian Religions
  • History of Religion
  • Judaism and Jewish Studies
  • Qumran Studies
  • Religion and Education
  • Religion and Health
  • Religion and Politics
  • Religion and Science
  • Religion and Law
  • Religion and Art, Literature, and Music
  • Religious Studies
  • Browse content in Society and Culture
  • Cookery, Food, and Drink
  • Cultural Studies
  • Customs and Traditions
  • Ethical Issues and Debates
  • Hobbies, Games, Arts and Crafts
  • Natural world, Country Life, and Pets
  • Popular Beliefs and Controversial Knowledge
  • Sports and Outdoor Recreation
  • Technology and Society
  • Travel and Holiday
  • Visual Culture
  • Browse content in Law
  • Arbitration
  • Browse content in Company and Commercial Law
  • Commercial Law
  • Company Law
  • Browse content in Comparative Law
  • Systems of Law
  • Competition Law
  • Browse content in Constitutional and Administrative Law
  • Government Powers
  • Judicial Review
  • Local Government Law
  • Military and Defence Law
  • Parliamentary and Legislative Practice
  • Construction Law
  • Contract Law
  • Browse content in Criminal Law
  • Criminal Procedure
  • Criminal Evidence Law
  • Sentencing and Punishment
  • Employment and Labour Law
  • Environment and Energy Law
  • Browse content in Financial Law
  • Banking Law
  • Insolvency Law
  • History of Law
  • Human Rights and Immigration
  • Intellectual Property Law
  • Browse content in International Law
  • Private International Law and Conflict of Laws
  • Public International Law
  • IT and Communications Law
  • Jurisprudence and Philosophy of Law
  • Law and Politics
  • Law and Society
  • Browse content in Legal System and Practice
  • Courts and Procedure
  • Legal Skills and Practice
  • Legal System - Costs and Funding
  • Primary Sources of Law
  • Regulation of Legal Profession
  • Medical and Healthcare Law
  • Browse content in Policing
  • Criminal Investigation and Detection
  • Police and Security Services
  • Police Procedure and Law
  • Police Regional Planning
  • Browse content in Property Law
  • Personal Property Law
  • Restitution
  • Study and Revision
  • Terrorism and National Security Law
  • Browse content in Trusts Law
  • Wills and Probate or Succession
  • Browse content in Medicine and Health
  • Browse content in Allied Health Professions
  • Arts Therapies
  • Clinical Science
  • Dietetics and Nutrition
  • Occupational Therapy
  • Operating Department Practice
  • Physiotherapy
  • Radiography
  • Speech and Language Therapy
  • Browse content in Anaesthetics
  • General Anaesthesia
  • Clinical Neuroscience
  • Browse content in Clinical Medicine
  • Acute Medicine
  • Cardiovascular Medicine
  • Clinical Genetics
  • Clinical Pharmacology and Therapeutics
  • Dermatology
  • Endocrinology and Diabetes
  • Gastroenterology
  • Genito-urinary Medicine
  • Geriatric Medicine
  • Infectious Diseases
  • Medical Toxicology
  • Medical Oncology
  • Pain Medicine
  • Palliative Medicine
  • Rehabilitation Medicine
  • Respiratory Medicine and Pulmonology
  • Rheumatology
  • Sleep Medicine
  • Sports and Exercise Medicine
  • Community Medical Services
  • Critical Care
  • Emergency Medicine
  • Forensic Medicine
  • Haematology
  • History of Medicine
  • Browse content in Medical Skills
  • Clinical Skills
  • Communication Skills
  • Nursing Skills
  • Surgical Skills
  • Browse content in Medical Dentistry
  • Oral and Maxillofacial Surgery
  • Paediatric Dentistry
  • Restorative Dentistry and Orthodontics
  • Surgical Dentistry
  • Medical Ethics
  • Medical Statistics and Methodology
  • Browse content in Neurology
  • Clinical Neurophysiology
  • Neuropathology
  • Nursing Studies
  • Browse content in Obstetrics and Gynaecology
  • Gynaecology
  • Occupational Medicine
  • Ophthalmology
  • Otolaryngology (ENT)
  • Browse content in Paediatrics
  • Neonatology
  • Browse content in Pathology
  • Chemical Pathology
  • Clinical Cytogenetics and Molecular Genetics
  • Histopathology
  • Medical Microbiology and Virology
  • Patient Education and Information
  • Browse content in Pharmacology
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Browse content in Popular Health
  • Caring for Others
  • Complementary and Alternative Medicine
  • Self-help and Personal Development
  • Browse content in Preclinical Medicine
  • Cell Biology
  • Molecular Biology and Genetics
  • Reproduction, Growth and Development
  • Primary Care
  • Professional Development in Medicine
  • Browse content in Psychiatry
  • Addiction Medicine
  • Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
  • Forensic Psychiatry
  • Learning Disabilities
  • Old Age Psychiatry
  • Psychotherapy
  • Browse content in Public Health and Epidemiology
  • Epidemiology
  • Public Health
  • Browse content in Radiology
  • Clinical Radiology
  • Interventional Radiology
  • Nuclear Medicine
  • Radiation Oncology
  • Reproductive Medicine
  • Browse content in Surgery
  • Cardiothoracic Surgery
  • Gastro-intestinal and Colorectal Surgery
  • General Surgery
  • Neurosurgery
  • Paediatric Surgery
  • Peri-operative Care
  • Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery
  • Surgical Oncology
  • Transplant Surgery
  • Trauma and Orthopaedic Surgery
  • Vascular Surgery
  • Browse content in Science and Mathematics
  • Browse content in Biological Sciences
  • Aquatic Biology
  • Biochemistry
  • Bioinformatics and Computational Biology
  • Developmental Biology
  • Ecology and Conservation
  • Evolutionary Biology
  • Genetics and Genomics
  • Microbiology
  • Molecular and Cell Biology
  • Natural History
  • Plant Sciences and Forestry
  • Research Methods in Life Sciences
  • Structural Biology
  • Systems Biology
  • Zoology and Animal Sciences
  • Browse content in Chemistry
  • Analytical Chemistry
  • Computational Chemistry
  • Crystallography
  • Environmental Chemistry
  • Industrial Chemistry
  • Inorganic Chemistry
  • Materials Chemistry
  • Medicinal Chemistry
  • Mineralogy and Gems
  • Organic Chemistry
  • Physical Chemistry
  • Polymer Chemistry
  • Study and Communication Skills in Chemistry
  • Theoretical Chemistry
  • Browse content in Computer Science
  • Artificial Intelligence
  • Computer Architecture and Logic Design
  • Game Studies
  • Human-Computer Interaction
  • Mathematical Theory of Computation
  • Programming Languages
  • Software Engineering
  • Systems Analysis and Design
  • Virtual Reality
  • Browse content in Computing
  • Business Applications
  • Computer Security
  • Computer Games
  • Computer Networking and Communications
  • Digital Lifestyle
  • Graphical and Digital Media Applications
  • Operating Systems
  • Browse content in Earth Sciences and Geography
  • Atmospheric Sciences
  • Environmental Geography
  • Geology and the Lithosphere
  • Maps and Map-making
  • Meteorology and Climatology
  • Oceanography and Hydrology
  • Palaeontology
  • Physical Geography and Topography
  • Regional Geography
  • Soil Science
  • Urban Geography
  • Browse content in Engineering and Technology
  • Agriculture and Farming
  • Biological Engineering
  • Civil Engineering, Surveying, and Building
  • Electronics and Communications Engineering
  • Energy Technology
  • Engineering (General)
  • Environmental Science, Engineering, and Technology
  • History of Engineering and Technology
  • Mechanical Engineering and Materials
  • Technology of Industrial Chemistry
  • Transport Technology and Trades
  • Browse content in Environmental Science
  • Applied Ecology (Environmental Science)
  • Conservation of the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Environmental Sustainability
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Environmental Science)
  • Management of Land and Natural Resources (Environmental Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environmental Science)
  • Nuclear Issues (Environmental Science)
  • Pollution and Threats to the Environment (Environmental Science)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Environmental Science)
  • History of Science and Technology
  • Browse content in Materials Science
  • Ceramics and Glasses
  • Composite Materials
  • Metals, Alloying, and Corrosion
  • Nanotechnology
  • Browse content in Mathematics
  • Applied Mathematics
  • Biomathematics and Statistics
  • History of Mathematics
  • Mathematical Education
  • Mathematical Finance
  • Mathematical Analysis
  • Numerical and Computational Mathematics
  • Probability and Statistics
  • Pure Mathematics
  • Browse content in Neuroscience
  • Cognition and Behavioural Neuroscience
  • Development of the Nervous System
  • Disorders of the Nervous System
  • History of Neuroscience
  • Invertebrate Neurobiology
  • Molecular and Cellular Systems
  • Neuroendocrinology and Autonomic Nervous System
  • Neuroscientific Techniques
  • Sensory and Motor Systems
  • Browse content in Physics
  • Astronomy and Astrophysics
  • Atomic, Molecular, and Optical Physics
  • Biological and Medical Physics
  • Classical Mechanics
  • Computational Physics
  • Condensed Matter Physics
  • Electromagnetism, Optics, and Acoustics
  • History of Physics
  • Mathematical and Statistical Physics
  • Measurement Science
  • Nuclear Physics
  • Particles and Fields
  • Plasma Physics
  • Quantum Physics
  • Relativity and Gravitation
  • Semiconductor and Mesoscopic Physics
  • Browse content in Psychology
  • Affective Sciences
  • Clinical Psychology
  • Cognitive Psychology
  • Cognitive Neuroscience
  • Criminal and Forensic Psychology
  • Developmental Psychology
  • Educational Psychology
  • Evolutionary Psychology
  • Health Psychology
  • History and Systems in Psychology
  • Music Psychology
  • Neuropsychology
  • Organizational Psychology
  • Psychological Assessment and Testing
  • Psychology of Human-Technology Interaction
  • Psychology Professional Development and Training
  • Research Methods in Psychology
  • Social Psychology
  • Browse content in Social Sciences
  • Browse content in Anthropology
  • Anthropology of Religion
  • Human Evolution
  • Medical Anthropology
  • Physical Anthropology
  • Regional Anthropology
  • Social and Cultural Anthropology
  • Theory and Practice of Anthropology
  • Browse content in Business and Management
  • Business Ethics
  • Business Strategy
  • Business History
  • Business and Technology
  • Business and Government
  • Business and the Environment
  • Comparative Management
  • Corporate Governance
  • Corporate Social Responsibility
  • Entrepreneurship
  • Health Management
  • Human Resource Management
  • Industrial and Employment Relations
  • Industry Studies
  • Information and Communication Technologies
  • International Business
  • Knowledge Management
  • Management and Management Techniques
  • Operations Management
  • Organizational Theory and Behaviour
  • Pensions and Pension Management
  • Public and Nonprofit Management
  • Social Issues in Business and Management
  • Strategic Management
  • Supply Chain Management
  • Browse content in Criminology and Criminal Justice
  • Criminal Justice
  • Criminology
  • Forms of Crime
  • International and Comparative Criminology
  • Youth Violence and Juvenile Justice
  • Development Studies
  • Browse content in Economics
  • Agricultural, Environmental, and Natural Resource Economics
  • Asian Economics
  • Behavioural Finance
  • Behavioural Economics and Neuroeconomics
  • Econometrics and Mathematical Economics
  • Economic History
  • Economic Systems
  • Economic Methodology
  • Economic Development and Growth
  • Financial Markets
  • Financial Institutions and Services
  • General Economics and Teaching
  • Health, Education, and Welfare
  • History of Economic Thought
  • International Economics
  • Labour and Demographic Economics
  • Law and Economics
  • Macroeconomics and Monetary Economics
  • Microeconomics
  • Public Economics
  • Urban, Rural, and Regional Economics
  • Welfare Economics
  • Browse content in Education
  • Adult Education and Continuous Learning
  • Care and Counselling of Students
  • Early Childhood and Elementary Education
  • Educational Equipment and Technology
  • Educational Strategies and Policy
  • Higher and Further Education
  • Organization and Management of Education
  • Philosophy and Theory of Education
  • Schools Studies
  • Secondary Education
  • Teaching of a Specific Subject
  • Teaching of Specific Groups and Special Educational Needs
  • Teaching Skills and Techniques
  • Browse content in Environment
  • Applied Ecology (Social Science)
  • Climate Change
  • Conservation of the Environment (Social Science)
  • Environmentalist Thought and Ideology (Social Science)
  • Management of Land and Natural Resources (Social Science)
  • Natural Disasters (Environment)
  • Pollution and Threats to the Environment (Social Science)
  • Social Impact of Environmental Issues (Social Science)
  • Sustainability
  • Browse content in Human Geography
  • Cultural Geography
  • Economic Geography
  • Political Geography
  • Browse content in Interdisciplinary Studies
  • Communication Studies
  • Museums, Libraries, and Information Sciences
  • Browse content in Politics
  • African Politics
  • Asian Politics
  • Chinese Politics
  • Comparative Politics
  • Conflict Politics
  • Elections and Electoral Studies
  • Environmental Politics
  • Ethnic Politics
  • European Union
  • Foreign Policy
  • Gender and Politics
  • Human Rights and Politics
  • Indian Politics
  • International Relations
  • International Organization (Politics)
  • Irish Politics
  • Latin American Politics
  • Middle Eastern Politics
  • Political Behaviour
  • Political Economy
  • Political Institutions
  • Political Methodology
  • Political Communication
  • Political Philosophy
  • Political Sociology
  • Political Theory
  • Politics and Law
  • Politics of Development
  • Public Policy
  • Public Administration
  • Qualitative Political Methodology
  • Quantitative Political Methodology
  • Regional Political Studies
  • Russian Politics
  • Security Studies
  • State and Local Government
  • UK Politics
  • US Politics
  • Browse content in Regional and Area Studies
  • African Studies
  • Asian Studies
  • East Asian Studies
  • Japanese Studies
  • Latin American Studies
  • Middle Eastern Studies
  • Native American Studies
  • Scottish Studies
  • Browse content in Research and Information
  • Research Methods
  • Browse content in Social Work
  • Addictions and Substance Misuse
  • Adoption and Fostering
  • Care of the Elderly
  • Child and Adolescent Social Work
  • Couple and Family Social Work
  • Direct Practice and Clinical Social Work
  • Emergency Services
  • Human Behaviour and the Social Environment
  • International and Global Issues in Social Work
  • Mental and Behavioural Health
  • Social Justice and Human Rights
  • Social Policy and Advocacy
  • Social Work and Crime and Justice
  • Social Work Macro Practice
  • Social Work Practice Settings
  • Social Work Research and Evidence-based Practice
  • Welfare and Benefit Systems
  • Browse content in Sociology
  • Childhood Studies
  • Community Development
  • Comparative and Historical Sociology
  • Disability Studies
  • Economic Sociology
  • Gender and Sexuality
  • Gerontology and Ageing
  • Health, Illness, and Medicine
  • Marriage and the Family
  • Migration Studies
  • Occupations, Professions, and Work
  • Organizations
  • Population and Demography
  • Race and Ethnicity
  • Social Theory
  • Social Movements and Social Change
  • Social Research and Statistics
  • Social Stratification, Inequality, and Mobility
  • Sociology of Religion
  • Sociology of Education
  • Sport and Leisure
  • Urban and Rural Studies
  • Browse content in Warfare and Defence
  • Defence Strategy, Planning, and Research
  • Land Forces and Warfare
  • Military Administration
  • Military Life and Institutions
  • Naval Forces and Warfare
  • Other Warfare and Defence Issues
  • Peace Studies and Conflict Resolution
  • Weapons and Equipment

Motivation Science: Controversies and Insights

  • < Previous
  • Next chapter >

Motivation Science: Controversies and Insights

Essay 1.1 What Is Motivation, Where Does It Come from, and How Does It Work?

  • Published: January 2023
  • Cite Icon Cite
  • Permissions Icon Permissions

Motivation is the process that drives, selects, and directs goals and behaviors. Motivation typically arises out of the person’s needs, and it then comes to life through the person’s specific goals. In this essay, the authors examine the concept of “needs” as the crucible from which motivated behavior arises because all individuals are born with needs that jump-start the goal-oriented, motivated behaviors that are critical to survival and thriving. These are both physical needs (such as hunger and thirst) and psychological needs (such as the need for social relationships, optimal predictability, and competence). The aim of motivation is therefore to bring about a desired (need, goal) state. Motivation underlies and organizes all aspects of a person’s psychology. As it does so, motivation “glues” a person together as a functioning individual in their culture and context.

Personal account

  • Sign in with email/username & password
  • Get email alerts
  • Save searches
  • Purchase content
  • Activate your purchase/trial code
  • Add your ORCID iD

Institutional access

Sign in with a library card.

  • Sign in with username/password
  • Recommend to your librarian
  • Institutional account management
  • Get help with access

Access to content on Oxford Academic is often provided through institutional subscriptions and purchases. If you are a member of an institution with an active account, you may be able to access content in one of the following ways:

IP based access

Typically, access is provided across an institutional network to a range of IP addresses. This authentication occurs automatically, and it is not possible to sign out of an IP authenticated account.

Choose this option to get remote access when outside your institution. Shibboleth/Open Athens technology is used to provide single sign-on between your institution’s website and Oxford Academic.

  • Click Sign in through your institution.
  • Select your institution from the list provided, which will take you to your institution's website to sign in.
  • When on the institution site, please use the credentials provided by your institution. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.
  • Following successful sign in, you will be returned to Oxford Academic.

If your institution is not listed or you cannot sign in to your institution’s website, please contact your librarian or administrator.

Enter your library card number to sign in. If you cannot sign in, please contact your librarian.

Society Members

Society member access to a journal is achieved in one of the following ways:

Sign in through society site

Many societies offer single sign-on between the society website and Oxford Academic. If you see ‘Sign in through society site’ in the sign in pane within a journal:

  • Click Sign in through society site.
  • When on the society site, please use the credentials provided by that society. Do not use an Oxford Academic personal account.

If you do not have a society account or have forgotten your username or password, please contact your society.

Sign in using a personal account

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members. See below.

A personal account can be used to get email alerts, save searches, purchase content, and activate subscriptions.

Some societies use Oxford Academic personal accounts to provide access to their members.

Viewing your signed in accounts

Click the account icon in the top right to:

  • View your signed in personal account and access account management features.
  • View the institutional accounts that are providing access.

Signed in but can't access content

Oxford Academic is home to a wide variety of products. The institutional subscription may not cover the content that you are trying to access. If you believe you should have access to that content, please contact your librarian.

For librarians and administrators, your personal account also provides access to institutional account management. Here you will find options to view and activate subscriptions, manage institutional settings and access options, access usage statistics, and more.

Our books are available by subscription or purchase to libraries and institutions.

Month: Total Views:
January 2023 28
February 2023 31
March 2023 35
April 2023 52
May 2023 42
June 2023 9
July 2023 17
August 2023 29
September 2023 57
October 2023 22
November 2023 28
December 2023 37
January 2024 106
February 2024 46
March 2024 52
April 2024 46
May 2024 61
June 2024 39
July 2024 20
August 2024 13
  • About Oxford Academic
  • Publish journals with us
  • University press partners
  • What we publish
  • New features  
  • Open access
  • Rights and permissions
  • Accessibility
  • Advertising
  • Media enquiries
  • Oxford University Press
  • Oxford Languages
  • University of Oxford

Oxford University Press is a department of the University of Oxford. It furthers the University's objective of excellence in research, scholarship, and education by publishing worldwide

  • Copyright © 2024 Oxford University Press
  • Cookie settings
  • Cookie policy
  • Privacy policy
  • Legal notice

This Feature Is Available To Subscribers Only

Sign In or Create an Account

This PDF is available to Subscribers Only

For full access to this pdf, sign in to an existing account, or purchase an annual subscription.

American Psychological Association Logo

Motivation Science

  • Read this journal
  • Journal snapshot
  • Advertising information

Journal scope statement

Motivation Science is a multi-disciplinary journal that publishes significant contributions to the study of motivation, broadly conceived.

The journal publishes papers on diverse aspects of, and approaches to, the science of motivation, including work carried out in all subfields of psychology, cognitive science, economics, sociology, management science, organizational science, neuroscience and political science.

Primarily, Motivation Science features empirical papers on motivational topics, although theoretical papers and reviews of the literature will also be considered.

Equity, diversity, and inclusion

Motivation Science supports equity, diversity, and inclusion (EDI) in its practices. More information on these initiatives is available under EDI Efforts .

Call for papers

  • Call for papers: General

Editor’s Choice

One article from each issue of Motivation Science will be highlighted as an “ Editor’s Choice ” article. Selection is based on the recommendations of the associate editors, the paper’s potential impact to the field, the distinction of expanding the contributors to, or the focus of, the science, or its discussion of an important future direction for science. Editor’s Choice articles are featured alongside articles from other APA published journals in a bi-weekly newsletter and are temporarily made freely available to newsletter subscribers.

Author and editor spotlights

Explore journal highlights : free article summaries, editor interviews and editorials, journal awards, mentorship opportunities, and more.

Prior to submission, please carefully read and follow the submission guidelines detailed below. Manuscripts that do not conform to the submission guidelines may be returned without review.

To submit to the Editorial Office of Guido H. E. Gendolla and Rex A. Wright, please submit manuscripts electronically through the Manuscript Submission Portal in Word Document format (.doc).

Guido H. E. Gendolla University of Geneva

Rex A. Wright University of Texas Dell School of Medicine & University of North Texas, United States

Prepare manuscripts according to the Publication Manual of the American Psychological Association using the 7 th edition. Manuscripts may be copyedited for bias-free language (see Chapter 5 of the Publication Manual ). APA Style and Grammar Guidelines for the 7 th edition are available.

Submit Manuscript

Motivation Science welcomes significant high-quality manuscripts reporting research on diverse aspects of, and approaches to, the science of motivation.

Manuscripts should fall into one of the following categories:

Original Research Articles

These are full-length manuscripts reporting empirical research that advances the comprehension of variables and processes that influence motivation and behavior. Research articles can report more than one empirical study but are not required to do so. Preference will be given to reports that present theory-driven research involving tests of clearly derived hypotheses and findings with straightforward and substantive implications. Replication studies are welcome if they provide conclusive results. There is no space limit for research articles, but manuscripts should typically range between 2,500 and 7,000 words, exclusive of references, figures, and tables.

Original Research Brief Reports

These are abbreviated manuscripts succinctly reporting empirical research that advances the comprehension of variables and processes that influence motivation and behavior. Original research brief reports should report one empirical study or a short series of studies with similar designs and methods. Preference will be given to reports that present theory-driven research involving tests of clearly derived hypotheses and findings with straightforward and substantive implications. Replication studies are welcome if they provide conclusive results. The space limit for brief reports is 2,500 words, exclusive of references, figures, and tables.

Conceptual Articles

These are full-length theoretical papers and literature reviews that can — but are not required to — apply meta-analytic techniques. Preference will be given to analyses and reviews that have straightforward and substantive implications. There is no space limit for conceptual articles, but manuscripts should typically rage between 2,500 and 7,000 words, exclusive of references, figures, and tables.

Conceptual Brief Reports

These are abbreviated theoretical papers and literature reviews that can — but are not required to — apply meta-analytic techniques. Preference will be given to analyses and reviews that have straightforward and substantive implications. Conceptual brief reports also can involve reactive commentary (e.g., to a research or conceptual article). The space limit for conceptual brief reports is 2,500 words, exclusive of references, figures, and tables.

Research Methods in Motivation Science Articles

These manuscripts are intended to draw attention to methodological developments relevant to the scientific study of motivation. They should aim to enhance the use of techniques and insights that advance motivation science and its application. Examples might include articles (1) that describe techniques for validating new research instruments, (2) that introduce new quantitative methods, or (3) that debate important methodological issues. Articles should be accessible to non-expert readers with doctoral level training and avoid use of unnecessary technical content. They also should make clear how their content advances theory and/or practice. Preference will be given to well-constructed and documented reports that have substantive implications. There is no space limit for Methods articles, but manuscripts should typically range between 2,500 and 7,000 words, exclusive of references, figures, and tables.

Out-of-the-Box Articles

These are brief contributions that might attract broad interest but do not fit neatly into preceding submission categories. Submissions will include no more than 2,500 words and can involve a variety of formats. Examples include short thought pieces, humor pieces, pieces concerned with history, and pieces concerned with contemporary issues. Contributions might involve personal profiles (e.g., of important figures), interviews, and even substantive fictional depictions.

Masked Review Policy

Masked review, which means that the identities of both authors and reviewers are masked, is optional for Motivation Science . Authors should note in their cover letters whether they have opted for masked or unmasked review.

Masked Review

Authors who desire masked review should make every effort to see that the manuscript itself contains no clues to their identities:

  • Authors should never use first person ( I, my, we, our ) when referring to a study conducted by the author(s) or when doing so reveals the authors' identities (e.g., "in our previous work, Johnson et al. (1998) reported that..."). Instead, references to the authors' work should be in third person (e.g., "Johnson et al. (1998) reported that...").
  • The authors' institutional affiliations should also be masked in the manuscript.
  • Include the title of the manuscript along with all authors' names and institutional affiliations in the cover letter.
  • The first page of the manuscript should omit the authors' names and affiliations but should include the title of the manuscript and the date it is submitted.
  • Responsibility for masking the manuscript rests with the authors; manuscripts will be returned to the author if not appropriately masked. If the manuscript is accepted, authors will be asked to make changes in wording so that the paper is no longer masked.
  • After masked review, please ensure that the final version for production includes a byline and full author note for typesetting.

Research Transparency and Openness

Motivation Science encourages both methodological and data transparency to ensure the reproducibility of research results. Thus, we ask authors to ensure their manuscripts meet certain standards aligned with APA Style Journal Article Reporting Standards (JARS). These items include:

  • Sample Size and Stopping Rules : Authors must describe the sample size, power, and precision, including:

▪ Intended sample size

▪ Achieved sample size, if different from the intended sample size

▪ Determination of sample size, including:

◦ Power analysis, or methods used to determine precision of parameter estimates

◦ Explanation of any interim analyses and stopping rules employed

  • Reporting the full methods  for empirical studies, including all manipulations, measures, and eventual data exclusions.
  • Reporting in the author note when data stem from related research.  APA Style stipulates that authors must include any disclosures of data stemming from related research in the author note. The original findings, if published, should be referenced in an in-text citation.

▪ If data stem from related research, authors should report:

◦ Whether the full methods are available, either as a citation to a published paper or hosted repository.

  • Ethical approval: authors must include IRB or related institution ethical approval for the reported research. For empirical studies, if no ethical approval was sought, authors must explain why.
  • Open Data: Authors for Motivation Science are encouraged to make their data and stimulus materials publicly available, if possible, by providing a link in their submission to a relevant data repository.

Making data and materials publicly available can increase the impact of the research, enabling future researchers to incorporate the original authors’ work in model testing, replication projects, and meta-analyses, in addition to increasing the transparency of the research.

Consideration for publication in Motivation Science does not require public posting, so it is at the author(s)’ discretion to decide what is best for their projects in terms of public data, materials, and conditions on their use.

Data Availability Statement :

Regardless of whether or not they choose to make the article data openly available, authors must include a Data Availability Statement in the author note. Authors must indicate whether the data and code reported in the manuscript will be made available or provide a reason for not sharing the data. The link to the permanent repository for the dataset and codebook (or the brief statement explaining why data are not being shared) must be included in the author note.

Please note that the APA Publication Manual (7th ed.) does note that researchers must make their data available to permit other qualified professionals to confirm the analyses and results, upon request. Therefore, making data openly available now may save the authors time later on.

Authors opting for masked review should ensure their datasets and supporting materials are anonymized prior to submission. The Open Science Framework provides instructions for creating anonymized links to data sets, codebooks, and relevant scripts or materials to protect the integrity of the masked review process.

Should the manuscript be accepted, links to the data set, codebook, and supporting materials (now made non-anonymized) should be included in the author note, per the Data Availability Statement requirements described above.

Authors should review the updated JARS for quantitative , qualitative , and mixed methods research before submitting. These standards offer ways to improve transparency in reporting to ensure that readers have the information necessary to evaluate the quality of the research and to facilitate collaboration and replication. For further resources, including flowcharts, see the Journal Article Reporting Standards (JARS) website .

Author contribution statements using CRediT

The APA  Publication Manual  (7th ed.)  stipulates that “authorship encompasses…not only persons who do the writing but also those who have made substantial scientific contributions to a study.” In the spirit of transparency and openness,  Motivation Science  has adopted the  Contributor Roles Taxonomy (CRediT)  to describe each author's individual contributions to the work. CRediT offers authors the opportunity to share an accurate and detailed description of their diverse contributions to a manuscript.

Submitting authors will be asked to identify the contributions of all authors at initial submission according to this taxonomy. If the manuscript is accepted for publication, the CRediT designations will be published as an Author Contributions Statement in the author note of the final article. All authors should have reviewed and agreed to their individual contribution(s) before submission.

CRediT includes 14 contributor roles, as described below:

  • Conceptualization:  Ideas; formulation or evolution of overarching research goals and aims.
  • Data curation:  Management activities to annotate (produce metadata), scrub data and maintain research data (including software code, where it is necessary for interpreting the data itself) for initial use and later reuse.
  • Formal analysis:  Application of statistical, mathematical, computational, or other formal techniques to analyze or synthesize study data.
  • Funding acquisition:  Acquisition of the financial support for the project leading to this publication.
  • Investigation:  Conducting a research and investigation process, specifically performing the experiments, or data/evidence collection.
  • Methodology:  Development or design of methodology; creation of models.
  • Project administration:  Management and coordination responsibility for the research activity planning and execution.
  • Resources:  Provision of study materials, reagents, materials, patients, laboratory samples, animals, instrumentation, computing resources, or other analysis tools.
  • Software:  Programming, software development; designing computer programs; implementation of the computer code and supporting algorithms; testing of existing code components.
  • Supervision:  Oversight and leadership responsibility for the research activity planning and execution, including mentorship external to the core team.
  • Validation:  Verification, whether as a part of the activity or separate, of the overall replication/reproducibility of results/experiments and other research outputs.
  • Visualization:  Preparation, creation and/or presentation of the published work, specifically visualization/data presentation.
  • Writing—original draft:  Preparation, creation and/or presentation of the published work, specifically writing the initial draft (including substantive translation).
  • Writing—review and editing:  Preparation, creation and/or presentation of the published work by those from the original research group, specifically critical review, commentary or revision—including pre- or post-publication stages.

Authors can claim credit for more than one contributor role, and the same role can be attributed to more than one author.

Manuscript Preparation

Review APA's Journal Manuscript Preparation Guidelines before submitting your article.

Double-space all copy. Other formatting instructions, as well as instructions on preparing tables, figures, references, metrics, and abstracts, appear in the Manual . Additional guidance on APA Style is available on the APA Style website .

Below are additional instructions regarding the preparation of display equations, computer code, and tables.

Display Equations

We strongly encourage you to use MathType (third-party software) or Equation Editor 3.0 (built into pre-2007 versions of Word) to construct your equations, rather than the equation support that is built into Word 2007 and Word 2010. Equations composed with the built-in Word 2007/Word 2010 equation support are converted to low-resolution graphics when they enter the production process and must be rekeyed by the typesetter, which may introduce errors.

To construct your equations with MathType or Equation Editor 3.0:

  • Go to the Text section of the Insert tab and select Object.
  • Select MathType or Equation Editor 3.0 in the drop-down menu.

If you have an equation that has already been produced using Microsoft Word 2007 or 2010 and you have access to the full version of MathType 6.5 or later, you can convert this equation to MathType by clicking on MathType Insert Equation. Copy the equation from Microsoft Word and paste it into the MathType box. Verify that your equation is correct, click File, and then click Update. Your equation has now been inserted into your Word file as a MathType Equation.

Use Equation Editor 3.0 or MathType only for equations or for formulas that cannot be produced as Word text using the Times or Symbol font.

Computer Code

Because altering computer code in any way (e.g., indents, line spacing, line breaks, page breaks) during the typesetting process could alter its meaning, we treat computer code differently from the rest of your article in our production process. To that end, we request separate files for computer code.

In Online Supplemental Material

We request that runnable source code be included as supplemental material to the article. For more information, visit Supplementing Your Article With Online Material .

In the Text of the Article

If you would like to include code in the text of your published manuscript, please submit a separate file with your code exactly as you want it to appear, using Courier New font with a type size of 8 points. We will make an image of each segment of code in your article that exceeds 40 characters in length. (Shorter snippets of code that appear in text will be typeset in Courier New and run in with the rest of the text.) If an appendix contains a mix of code and explanatory text, please submit a file that contains the entire appendix, with the code keyed in 8-point Courier New.

Use Word's Insert Table function when you create tables. Using spaces or tabs in your table will create problems when the table is typeset and may result in errors.

Academic Writing and English Language Editing Services

Authors who feel that their manuscript may benefit from additional academic writing or language editing support prior to submission are encouraged to seek out such services at their host institutions, engage with colleagues and subject matter experts, and/or consider several vendors that offer discounts to APA authors .

Please note that APA does not endorse or take responsibility for the service providers listed. It is strictly a referral service.

Use of such service is not mandatory for publication in an APA journal. Use of one or more of these services does not guarantee selection for peer review, manuscript acceptance, or preference for publication in any APA journal.

Submitting Supplemental Materials

APA can place supplemental materials online, available via the published article in the APA PsycArticles ® database. Please see Supplementing Your Article With Online Material for more details.

Abstract and Keywords

All manuscripts must include an abstract containing a maximum of 250 words typed on a separate page. After the abstract, please supply up to five keywords or brief phrases.

List references in alphabetical order. Each listed reference should be cited in text, and each text citation should be listed in the References section.

Examples of basic reference formats:

Journal Article

McCauley, S. M., & Christiansen, M. H. (2019). Language learning as language use: A cross-linguistic model of child language development. Psychological Review , 126 (1), 1–51. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000126

Authored Book

Brown, L. S. (2018). Feminist therapy (2nd ed.). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/0000092-000

Chapter in an Edited Book

Balsam, K. F., Martell, C. R., Jones. K. P., & Safren, S. A. (2019). Affirmative cognitive behavior therapy with sexual and gender minority people. In G. Y. Iwamasa & P. A. Hays (Eds.), Culturally responsive cognitive behavior therapy: Practice and supervision (2nd ed., pp. 287–314). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/0000119-012

Preferred formats for graphics files are TIFF and JPG, and preferred format for vector-based files is EPS. Graphics downloaded or saved from web pages are not acceptable for publication. Multipanel figures (i.e., figures with parts labeled a, b, c, d, etc.) should be assembled into one file. When possible, please place symbol legends below the figure instead of to the side.

  • All color line art and halftones: 300 DPI
  • Black and white line tone and gray halftone images: 600 DPI

Line weights

  • Color (RGB, CMYK) images: 2 pixels
  • Grayscale images: 4 pixels
  • Stroke weight: 0.5 points

APA offers authors the option to publish their figures online in color without the costs associated with print publication of color figures.

The same caption will appear on both the online (color) and print (black and white) versions. To ensure that the figure can be understood in both formats, authors should add alternative wording (e.g., “the red (dark gray) bars represent”) as needed.

For authors who prefer their figures to be published in color both in print and online, original color figures can be printed in color at the editor's and publisher's discretion provided the author agrees to pay:

  • $900 for one figure
  • An additional $600 for the second figure
  • An additional $450 for each subsequent figure

Permissions

Authors of accepted papers must obtain and provide to the editor on final acceptance all necessary permissions to reproduce in print and electronic form any copyrighted work, including test materials (or portions thereof), photographs, and other graphic images (including those used as stimuli in experiments).

On advice of counsel, APA may decline to publish any image whose copyright status is unknown.

  • Download Permissions Alert Form (PDF, 13KB)

Publication Policies

For full details on publication policies, including use of Artificial Intelligence tools, please see APA Publishing Policies .

APA policy prohibits an author from submitting the same manuscript for concurrent consideration by two or more publications.

See also APA Journals ® Internet Posting Guidelines .

APA requires authors to reveal any possible conflict of interest in the conduct and reporting of research (e.g., financial interests in a test or procedure, funding by pharmaceutical companies for drug research).

  • Download Full Disclosure of Interests Form (PDF, 41KB)

Ethical Principles

It is a violation of APA Ethical Principles to publish "as original data, data that have been previously published" (Standard 8.13).

In addition, APA Ethical Principles specify that "after research results are published, psychologists do not withhold the data on which their conclusions are based from other competent professionals who seek to verify the substantive claims through reanalysis and who intend to use such data only for that purpose, provided that the confidentiality of the participants can be protected and unless legal rights concerning proprietary data preclude their release" (Standard 8.14).

APA expects authors to adhere to these standards. Specifically, APA expects authors to have their data available throughout the editorial review process and for at least 5 years after the date of publication.

Authors are required to state in writing that they have complied with APA ethical standards in the treatment of their sample, human or animal, or to describe the details of treatment.

  • Download Certification of Compliance With APA Ethical Principles Form (PDF, 26KB)

The APA Ethics Office provides the full Ethical Principles of Psychologists and Code of Conduct electronically on its website in HTML, PDF, and Word format. You may also request a copy by emailing or calling the APA Ethics Office (202-336-5930). You may also read "Ethical Principles," December 1992, American Psychologist , Vol. 47, pp. 1597–1611.

Other Information

See APA’s Publishing Policies page for more information on publication policies, including information on author contributorship and responsibilities of authors, author name changes after publication, the use of generative artificial intelligence, funder information and conflict-of-interest disclosures, duplicate publication, data publication and reuse, and preprints.

Visit the Journals Publishing Resource Center for more resources for writing, reviewing, and editing articles for publishing in APA journals.

Guido H. E. Gendolla, PhD University of Geneva, Switzerland

Rex A. Wright, PhD University of Texas Dell School of Medicine & University of North Texas, United States

Associate editors

Marylène Gagné, PhD Curtin University, Australia

Catalina Kopetz, PhD Wayne State University, United States

Joyce S. Pang, PhD Nanyang Technological University, Singapore

Consulting editors

Nicola Baumann, PhD University of Trier, Germany

Roy F. Baumeister, PhD University of Queensland, Australia, & Florida State University, United States

Erik Bijleveld, PhD Radboud University, the Netherlands

Veronika Brandstätter, PhD University of Zurich, Switzerland

C. Miguel Brendl, PhD University of Basel, Switzerland

Marcus Crede, PhD Iowa State University, United States

Ruud Custers, PhD Utrecht University, the Netherlands

David Dunning, PhD University of Michigan, United States

Jacquelynne Sue Eccles, PhD University of California, Irvine, United States

Andrew J. Elliot, PhD University of Rochester, United States

Ayelet Fishbach, PhD University of Chicago, United States

Alexandra M. Freund, PhD University of Zurich, Switzerland

Malte Friese, PhD Saarland University, Germany

James W. Fryer, PhD State University of New York at Potsdam, United States

Philip A. Gable, PhD University of Delaware, United States

Peter M. Gollwitzer, PhD New York University, United States

Ana Guinote, PhD University College London, United Kingdom

Martin S. Hagger, PhD University of California, Merced, United States, & University of Jyväskylä, Finland

Judith M. Harackiewicz, PhD University of Wisconsin – Madison, United States

Eddie Harmon-Jones, PhD University of New South Wales, Australia

Jutta Heckhausen, PhD University of California at Irvine, United States

Marlone Henderson, PhD University of Texas at Austin, United States

Marie Hennecke, PhD Ruhr-University Bochum, Germany

Thomas M. Hess, PhD North Carolina State University, United States

Suzanne E. Hidi, PhD University of Toronto, Canada

E. Tory Higgins, PhD Columbia University, United States

Wilhelm Hofmann, PhD University of Cologne, Germany

Bernhard Hommel, PhD Leiden University, the Netherlands

Julie Y. Huang, PhD Stony Brook University, United States

Veronika Job, PhD University of Zurich, Switzerland

John T. Jost, PhD New York University, United States

Miguel Kazén, PhD University of Osnabrück, Germany

Hugo Martin Kehr, PhD Technical University Munich School of Management, Germany

Daria Knoch, PhD University of Bern, Switzerland

Richard Koestner, PhD McGill University

Sander L. Koole, PhD Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, the Netherlands

Małgorzata Kossowska, Prof Jagiellonian University, Poland

Arie Wladimir Kruglanski, PhD University of Maryland, United States

Gary Latham, PhD University of Toronto, Canada

Edwin A. Locke, PhD University of Maryland, United States

Samuele Marcora, PhD University of Kent, United Kingdom

Marina Milyavskaya, PhD Carleton University, Canada

Gordon Moskowitz, PhD Lehigh University, United States

Nikos Ntoumanis, PhD Curtin University, Australia

Gabriele Oettingen, PhD New York University, United States

Giuseppe Pantaleo, PhD San Raffaele University of Milan, Italy

Erika A. Patall, PhD USC Rossier School of Education, United States

Mathias Pessiglione, PhD Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle Epinière, France

Luiz Pessoa, PhD University of Maryland at College Park, United States

Deborah A. Prentice, PhD Princeton University, United States

Johnmarshall Reeve, PhD Australian Catholic University, Australia

K. Ann Renninger, PhD Swarthmore College, United States

Michael Richter, PhD Liverpool John Moores University, United Kingdom

Richard M. Ryan, PhD Institute for Positive Psychology and Education at Australian Catholic University, Australia

Kaspar Schattke, DPhil Université du Québec à Montréal, Canada

Brandon J. Schmeichel, PhD Texas A&M University, United States

Abigail A. Scholer, PhD University of Waterloo, Canada

Oliver C. Schultheiss, DPhil Friedrich-Alexander University, Germany

Paschal Sheeran, PhD University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, United States

Kennon M. Sheldon, PhD University of Missouri, United States

Paul J. Silvia, PhD University of North Carolina at Greensboro, United States

Robert J. Vallerand, PhD Université du Québec à Montréal, Canada

Lotte Frederike van Dillen, PhD Leiden University, the Netherlands

Jack van Honk, PhD Utrecht University, the Netherlands

Nico W. Van Yperen, PhD University of Groningen, the Netherlands

Thomas L. Webb, PhD The University of Sheffield, United Kingdom

Bernard Weiner, PhD University of California at Los Angeles, United States

Abstracting and indexing services providing coverage of Motivation Science

  • APA PsycInfo

Special issue of the APA journal Motivation Science, Vol. 8, No. 2, June 2022. This special issue collection represents an effort to further expand our knowledge and understanding of aggression from a multidisciplinary perspective.

Special issue of the APA journal Motivation Science, Vol. 3, No. 3, September 2017. The issue presents a set of diverse papers that examine the topic of motivation from multiple points of view.

Inclusive study designs

  • Collaborative research models
  • Diverse samples

Definitions and further details on inclusive study designs are available on the Journals EDI homepage .

Inclusive reporting standards

  • Bias-free language and community-driven language guidelines (required)
  • Data sharing and data availability statements (required)
  • Sample justifications (required)

More information on this journal’s reporting standards is listed under the submission guidelines tab .

Other EDI offerings

Masked peer review.

This journal offers masked peer review (where both the authors’ and reviewers’ identities are not known to the other). Research has shown that masked peer review can help reduce implicit bias against traditionally female names or early-career scientists with smaller publication records (Budden et al., 2008; Darling, 2015).

  • September 2022: A Fresh Term of Service at Motivation Science (PDF, 130KB)
  • March 2020: Updates and News From Motivation Science
  • March 2018: Updates on the Development of Motivation Science
  • September 2016: Gathering the Diaspora: Aims and Visions for Motivation Science (PDF, 17KB)

Editor Spotlight

  • Read an interview with Editors Guido Gendolla, PhD and Rex Wright, PhD

From APA Journals Article Spotlight ®

  • Is seeing doing? How observing action outcomes may trigger behavior.
  • Saying Goodbye and Saying it Well

Journal Alert

Sign up to receive email alerts on the latest content published.

Welcome! Thank you for subscribing.

Subscriptions and access

  • Pricing and individual access
  • APA PsycArticles database

Calls for Papers

Access options

  • APA publishing resources
  • Educators and students
  • Editor resource center

APA Publishing Insider

APA Publishing Insider is a free monthly newsletter with tips on APA Style, open science initiatives, active calls for papers, research summaries, and more.

Social media

Twitter icon

Contact Journals

Motivation: Introduction to the Theory, Concepts, and Research

  • First Online: 03 May 2018

Cite this chapter

what is motivation in a research paper

  • Paulina Arango 4  

Part of the book series: Literacy Studies ((LITS,volume 15))

1828 Accesses

2 Citations

Motivation is a psychological construct that refers to the disposition to act and direct behavior according to a goal. Like most of psychological processes, motivation develops throughout the life span and is influenced by both biological and environmental factors. The aim of this chapter is to summarize research on the development of motivation from infancy to adolescence, which can help understand the typical developmental trajectories of this ability and its relation to learning. We will start with a review of some of the most influential theories of motivation and the aspects each of them has emphasized. We will also explore how biology and experience interact in this development, paying special attention to factors such as: school, family, and peers, as well as characteristics of the child including self-esteem, cognitive development, and temperament. Finally, we will discuss the implications of understanding the developmental trajectories and the factors that have an impact on this development, for both teachers and parents.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this chapter

Subscribe and save.

  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
  • Available as PDF
  • Read on any device
  • Instant download
  • Own it forever
  • Available as EPUB and PDF
  • Compact, lightweight edition
  • Dispatched in 3 to 5 business days
  • Free shipping worldwide - see info
  • Durable hardcover edition

Tax calculation will be finalised at checkout

Purchases are for personal use only

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

what is motivation in a research paper

Understanding and Supporting Student Motivation for Learning

what is motivation in a research paper

Motivation and Action: Introduction and Overview

what is motivation in a research paper

Intrinsic Motivation

This is not intended to be an exhaustive review of motivational theories. For a more detailed review see: (Dörnyei and Ushioda 2013 ; Eccles and Wigfield 2002 ; Wentzel and Miele 2009 ; Wigfield et al. 2007 ).

For more information on the development of motivation in adults you can see: Carstensen 1993 ; Kanfer and Ackerman 2004 ; Wlodkowski 2011 .

Atkinson, J. W. (1957). Motivational determinants of risk taking behavior. Psychological Review, 64 (6), 359–372. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043445 .

Article   Google Scholar  

Atkinson, J. W., & Raynor, J. O. (1978). Personality, motivation, and achievement . Oxford: Hemisphere.

Google Scholar  

Aunola, K., Leskinen, E., Onatsu-Arvilommi, T., & Nurmi, J. E. (2002). Three methods for studying developmental change: A case of reading skills and self-concept. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72 (3), 343–364. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709902320634447 .

Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory . Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

Bandura, A. (1991). Self-regulation of motivation through anticipatory and self-reactive mechanisms. In R. A. Dienstbier (Ed.), Perspectives on motivation: Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 38, pp. 69–164). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Bandura, A. (1997). Self-efficacy: The exercise of control . New York: Freeman.

Bandura, A. (1999). A social cognitive theory of personality. In L. Pervin & O. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality (2nd ed., pp. 154–196). New York: Guilford.

Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (2001). Self-efficacy beliefs as shapers of children’s aspirations and career trajectories. Child Development, 72 (1), 187–206. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00273 .

Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 10 (3), 295–307. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/10.3.295 .

Boulton, M. J., Don, J., & Boulton, L. (2011). Predicting children’s liking of school from their peer relationships. Social Psychology of Education, 14 (4), 489–501. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-011-9156-0 .

Bradley, R. H., & Corwyn, R. F. (2002). Socioeconomic status and child development. Annual Review of Psychology, 53 (1), 371–399. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135233 .

Brechwald, W. A., & Prinstein, M. J. (2011). Beyond homophily: A decade of advances in understanding peer influence processes. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21 (1), 166–179. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00721.x .

Buss, D. M. (2008). Human nature and individual differences. In S. E. Hampson & H. S. Friedman (Eds.), The handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 29–60). New York: The Guilford Press.

Cain, K. M., & Dweck, C. S. (1989). The development of children’s conceptions of Intelligence; A theoretical framework. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 5, pp. 47–82). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

Cain, K., & Dweck, C. S. (1995). The relation between motivational patterns and achievement cognitions through the elementary school years. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 41 (1), 25–52.

Carlson, C. L., Mann, M., & Alexander, D. K. (2000). Effects of reward and response cost on the performance and motivation of children with ADHD. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 24 (1), 87–98. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1005455009154 .

Carstensen, L. L. (1993, January). Motivation for social contact across the life span: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. In Nebraska symposium on motivation (Vol. 40, pp. 209–254).

Catalano, R. F., Berglund, M. L., Ryan, J. A., Lonczak, H. S., & Hawkins, J. D. (2004). Positive youth development in the United States: Research findings on evaluations of positive youth development programs. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 591 (1), 98–124. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002716203260102 .

Coll, C. G., Bearer, E. L., & Lerner, R. M. (2014). Nature and nurture: The complex interplay of genetic and environmental influences on human behavior and development . Mahwah: Psychology Press.

Collins, W. A., Maccoby, E. E., Steinberg, L., Hetherington, E. M., & Bornstein, M. H. (2000). Contemporary research on parenting: The case for nature and nurture. American Psychologist, 55 (2), 218–232. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003–066X.55.2.218 .

Conger, R. D., Wallace, L. E., Sun, Y., Simons, R. L., McLoyd, V. C., & Brody, G. H. (2002). Economic pressure in African American families: A replication and extension of the family stress model. Developmental Psychology, 38 (2), 179. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012–1649.38.2.179 .

Connell, J. P. (1985). A new multidimensional measure of children’s perception of control. Child Development, 56 (4), 1018–1041. https://doi.org/10.2307/1130113 .

Damasio, A. R., Everitt, B. J., & Bishop, D. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex [and discussion]. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 351 (1346), 1413–1420. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.1996.0125 .

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2000). The “what” and “why” of goal pursuits: Human needs and the self–determination of behavior. Psychological Inquiry, 11 (4), 227–268. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327965PLI1104_01 .

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2002a). The paradox of achievement: The harder you push, the worse it gets. In J. Aronson (Ed.), Improving academic achievement: Impact of psychological factors on education (pp. 61–87). San Diego: Academic Press.

Chapter   Google Scholar  

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2002b). Self–determination research: Reflections and future directions. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self–determination theory research (pp. 431–441). Rochester: University of Rochester Press.

Dörnyei, Z., & Ushioda, E. (2013). Teaching and researching: Motivation . London: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315833750 .

Book   Google Scholar  

Dweck, C. S. (2002). The development of ability conceptions. In A. Wigfield & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Development of achievement motivation (pp. 57–88). San Diego: Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978–012750053–9/50005–X .

Eccles, J. S. (1987). Gender roles and women’s achievement–related decisions. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11 (2), 135–172. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1471–6402.1987.tb00781.x .

Eccles, J. S. (1993). School and family effects on the ontogeny of children’s interests, self–perceptions, and activity choice. In J. Jacobs (Ed.), Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1992: Developmental perspectives on motivation (pp. 145–208). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press.

Eccles, J. S., & Harold, R. D. (1993). Parent–school involvement during the early adolescent years. Teachers’ College Record, 94 , 568–587.

Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (1995). In the mind of the actor: The structure of adolescents’ achievement task values and expectancy–related beliefs. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21 (3), 215–225. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167295213003 .

Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values, and goals. Annual Review of Psychology, 53 (1), 109–132. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135153 .

Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., Harold, R., & Blumenfeld, P. B. (1993). Age and gender differences in children’s self– And task perceptions during elementary school. Child Development, 64 (3), 830–847. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–8624.1993.tb02946.x .

Eccles, J. S., Wigfield, A., & Schiefele, U. (1998). Motivation to succeed. In W. Damon (Series Ed.) & N. Eisenberg (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of Child Psychology (5th ed. Vol. 3, pp. 1017–1095). New York: Wiley.

Eccles–Parsons, J., Adler, T. F., & Kaczala, C. M. (1982). Socialization of achievement attitudes and beliefs: Parental influences. Child Development, 53 (2), 310–321. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128973 .

Eccles–Parsons, J., Adler, T. F., Futterman, R., Goff, S. B., Kaczala, C. M., Meece, J. L., et al. (1983). Expectancies, values, and academic behaviors. In J. T. Spence (Ed.), Achievement and achievement motivation (pp. 75–146). San Francisco: Freeman.

Eggen, P. D., & Kauchak, D. P. (2007). Educational psychology: Windows on classrooms . Virginia: Prentice Hall.

Ernst, M. (2014). The triadic model perspective for the study of adolescent motivated behavior. Brain and Cognition, 89 , 104–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2014.01.006 .

Ernst, M., & Fudge, J. L. (2009). A developmental neurobiological model of motivated behavior: Anatomy, connectivity and ontogeny of the triadic nodes. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33 (3), 367–382. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.10.009 .

Ernst, M., & Paulus, M. P. (2005). Neurobiology of decision making: A selective review from a neurocognitive and clinical perspective. Biological Psychiatry, 58 (8), 597–604. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.004 .

Ernst, M., Pine, D. S., & Hardin, M. (2006). Triadic model of the neurobiology of motivated behavior in adolescence. Psychological Medicine, 36 (03), 299–312. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291705005891 .

Ernst, M., Romeo, R. D., & Andersen, S. L. (2009). Neurobiology of the development of motivated behaviors in adolescence: A window into a neural systems model. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 93 (3), 199–211. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbb.2008.12.013 .

Fredricks, J. A., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). Children’s competence and value beliefs from childhood through adolescence: Growth trajectories in two male–sex–typed domains. Developmental Psychology, 38 (4), 519–533. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012–1649.38.4.519 .

Fredricks, J. A., & Eccles, J. S. (2004). Parental influences on youth involvement in sports. In M. R. Weiss (Ed.), Developmental sport and exercise psychology: A lifespan perspective (pp. 145–164). Morgantown: Fitness Information Technology.

Freud, S. (1920). Beyond the pleasure principle . London: The Hogarth Press and the Institute of Psychoanalysis.

Friedel, J. M., Cortina, K. S., Turner, J. C., & Midgley, C. (2007). Achievement goals, efficacy beliefs and coping strategies in mathematics: The roles of perceived parent and teacher goal emphases. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32 (3), 434–458. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2006.10.009 .

Furrer, C., & Skinner, E. (2003). Sense of relatedness as a factor in children’s academic engagement and performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95 (1), 148–162. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022–0663.95.1.148 .

Gallardo, L. O., Barrasa, A., & Guevara–Viejo, F. (2016). Positive peer relationships and academic achievement across early and midadolescence. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 44 (10), 1637–1648. https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2016.44.10.1637 .

Glenn, S., Dayus, B., Cunningham, C., & Horgan, M. (2001). Mastery motivation in children with down syndrome. Down Syndrome Research and Practice, 7 (2), 52–59. https://doi.org/10.3104/reports.114 .

Gniewosz, B., Eccles, J. S., & Noack, P. (2015). Early adolescents’ development of academic self-concept and intrinsic task value: The role of contextual feedback. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 25 (3), 459–473. https://doi.org/10.1111/jora.12140 .

Gunderson, E. A., Ramirez, G., Levine, S. C., & Beilock, S. L. (2012). The role of parents and teachers in the development of gender–related math attitudes. Sex Roles, 66 (3–4), 153–166. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-011-9996-2 .

Guthrie, J. T., Wigfield, A., & Perencevich, K. C. (Eds.). (2004). Motivating reading comprehension: Concept oriented reading instruction . Mahwah: Erlbaum. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410610126 .

Harackiewicz, J. M., Barron, K. E., & Elliot, A. J. (1998). Rethinking achievement goals: When are they adaptive for college students and why? Educational Psychologist, 33 (1), 1–21. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep3301_1 .

Heckhausen, H. (1987). Emotional components of action: Their ontogeny as reflected in achievement behavior. In D. Gîrlitz & J. F. Wohlwill (Eds.), Curiosity, imagination, and play (pp. 326–348). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

Heckhausen, J. (2000). Motivational psychology of human development: Developing motivation and motivating development . Amsterdam: Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4115(00)80003-5 .

Hokoda, A., & Fincham, F. D. (1995). Origins of children’s helpless and mastery achievement patterns in the family. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87 (3), 375–385. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.87.3.375 .

Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior: An introduction to behavior theory . Oxford: Appleton-Century Company, Incorporated.

Jacobs, J. E., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Parents, task values, and real-life achievement-related choices. In C. Sansone & J. M. Harackiewicz (Eds.), Intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: The search for optimal motivation and performance (pp. 405–439). San Diego: Academic Press.

Jacobs, J. E., Lanza, S., Osgood, D. W., Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Changes in children’s self-competence and values: Gender and domain differences across grades one through twelve. Child Development, 73 (2), 509–527. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00421 .

Jacobs, J. E., Davis-Kean, P., Bleeker, M., Eccles, J. S., & Malanchuk, O. (2005). I can, but I don’t want to. The impact of parents, interests, and activities on gender differences in math. In A. Gallagher & J. Kaufman (Eds.), Gender differences in mathematics: An integrative psychological approach (pp. 246–263). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

James, W. (1963). Psychology . New York: Fawcett.

Kahne, J., Nagaoka, J., Brown, A., O’Brien, J., Quinn, T., & Thiede, K. (2001). Assessing after-school programs as contexts for youth development. Youth & Society, 32 (4), 421–446. https://doi.org/10.1177/0044118X01032004002 .

Kanfer, R., & Ackerman, P. L. (2004). Aging, adult development, and work motivation. Academy of Management Review, 29 (3), 440–458. https://doi.org/10.5465/AMR.2004.13670969 .

Karbach, J., Gottschling, J., Spengler, M., Hegewald, K., & Spinath, F. M. (2013). Parental involvement and general cognitive ability as predictors of domain-specific academic achievement in early adolescence. Learning and Instruction, 23 , 43–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.09.004 .

King, R. B., & Ganotice, F. A., Jr. (2014). The social underpinnings of motivation and achievement: Investigating the role of parents, teachers, and peers on academic outcomes. The Asia-Pacific Education Researcher, 23 (3), 745–756. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40299-013-0148-z .

Kleinginna, P. R., Jr., & Kleinginna, A. M. (1981). A categorized list of motivation definitions, with a suggestion for a consensual definition. Motivation and Emotion, 5 (3), 263–291. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00993889 .

Lee, J., & Shute, V. J. (2010). Personal and social-contextual factors in K–12 academic performance: An integrative perspective on student learning. Educational Psychologist, 45 (3), 185–202. 1080/00461520.2010.493471 .

Lee, V. E., & Smith, J. (2001). Restructuring high schools for equity and excellence: What works . New York: Teachers College Press.

Linnenbrink, E. A. (2005). The dilemma of performance-approach goals: The use of multiple goal contexts to promote students’ motivation and learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97 (2), 197–213. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.97.2.197 .

Logan, M., & Skamp, K. (2008). Engaging students in science across the primary secondary interface: Listening to the students’ voice. Research in Science Education, 38 (4), 501–527. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11165-007-9063-8 .

Mantzicopoulos, P., French, B. F., & Maller, S. J. (2004). Factor structure of the pictorial scale of perceived competence and social acceptance with two pre-elementary samples. Child Development, 75 (4), 1214–1228. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00734.x .

Marjoribanks, K. (2002). Family and school capital: Towards a context theory of students’ school outcomes . Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-015-9980-1 .

Maslow, A. H. (1943). A theory of human motivation. Psychological Review, 50 (4), 370–396. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054346 .

McDougal, W. (1908). An introduction to social psychology . Boston: John W. Luce and Co..

McInerney, D. M. (2008). Personal investment, culture and learning: Insights into school achievement across Anglo, aboriginal, Asian and Lebanese students in Australia. International Journal of Psychology, 43 (5), 870–879. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207590701836364 .

Midgley, C. (Ed.). (2014). Goals, goal structures, and patterns of adaptive learning . Abingdon: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781410602152 .

Murdock, T. B., Hale, N. M., & Weber, M. J. (2001). Predictors of cheating among early adolescents: Academic and social motivations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 26 (1), 96–115. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.2000.1046 .

National Research Council. (2004). Engaging schools: Fostering high school students’ motivation to learn . Washington, DC: National Academies Press. https://doi.org/10.5860/choice.42–1079 .

Nelson, R. M., & DeBacker, T. K. (2008). Achievement motivation in adolescents: The role of peer climate and best friends. The Journal of Experimental Education, 76 (2), 170–189. https://doi.org/10.3200/JEXE.76.2.170-190 .

Niccols, A., Atkinson, L., & Pepler, D. (2003). Mastery motivation in young children with Down’s syndrome: Relations with cognitive and adaptive competence. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 47 (2), 121–133. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2788.2003.00452.x .

Nicholls, J. G. (1979). Development of perception of own attainment and causal attributions for success and failure in reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 71 (1), 94–99. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.71.1.94 .

Nicholls, J. G., & Miller, A. T. (1984). The differentiation of the concepts of difficulty and ability. Child Development, 54 (4), 951–959. https://doi.org/10.2307/1129899 .

Patrick, H., Anderman, L. H., Ryan, A. M., Edelin, K. C., & Midgley, C. (2001). Teachers’ communication of goal orientations in four fifth-grade classrooms. The Elementary School Journal, 102 (1), 35–58. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/1002168 .

Pavlov, I. P. (2003). Conditioned reflexes . Mineola: Courier Corporation.

Pesu, L. A., Aunola, K., Viljaranta, J., & Nurmi, J. E. (2016a). The development of adolescents’ self-concept of ability through grades 7–9 and the role of parental beliefs. Frontline Learning Research, 4 (3), 92–109. https://doi.org/10.14786/flr.v4i3.249 .

Pesu, L., Viljaranta, J., & Aunola, K. (2016b). The role of parents’ and teachers’ beliefs in children’s self-concept development. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 44 , 63–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2016.03.001 .

Petri, H. L., & Govern, J. M. (2012). Motivation: Theory, research, and application . Belmont: Wadsworth Publishing.

Pintrich, P. R. (2000). Multiple goals, multiple pathways: The role of goal orientation in learning and achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92 (3), 544–555. 10.I037//0022-O663.92.3.544 .

Pintrich, P. R. (2003). A motivational science perspective on the role of student motivation in learning and teaching contexts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95 (4), 667. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.667 .

Pintrich, P. R., & Maehr, M. L. (2004). Advances in motivation and achievement: Motivating students, improving schools (Vol. 13). Bingley: Emerald Grup Publishing Limited.

Ratelle, C. F., Guay, F., Larose, S., & Senécal, C. (2004). Family correlates of trajectories of academic motivation during a school transition: A semiparametric group-based approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96 (4), 743. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.743 .

Roeser, R. W., Eccles, J. S., & Sameroff, A. J. (1998). Academic and emotional functioning in early adolescence: Longitudinal relations, patterns, and prediction by experience in middle school. Development and Psychopathology, 10 (02), 321–352.

Roeser, R. W., Marachi, R., & Gelhbach, H. (2002). A goal theory perspective on teachers’ professional identities and the contexts of teaching. In C. M. Midgley (Ed.), Goals, goal structures, and patterns of adaptive learning (pp. 205–241). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

Rotter, J. B. (1966). Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 80 (1), 1–28. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0092976 .

Ryan, A. M. (2001). The peer group as a context for the development of young adolescents’ motivation and achievement. Child Development, 72 (4), 1135–1150. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00338 .

Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2002). An overview of self-determination theory: An organismic-dialectical perspective. In E. L. Deci & R. M. Ryan (Eds.), Handbook of self-determination theory research (pp. 3–33). Rochester: University of Rochester Press.

Schunk, D. H., & Pajares, F. (2002). The development of academic self-efficacy. In A. Wigfield & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Development of achievement motivation (pp. 15–32). San Diego: Academic Press.

Schunk, D. H., Meece, J. R., & Pintrich, P. R. (2012). Motivation in education: Theory, research, and applications . Harlow: Pearson Higher Ed.

Shell, D. F., Colvin, C., & Bruning, R. H. (1995). Self-efficacy, attribution, and outcome expectancy mechanisms in reading and writing achievement: Grade-level and achievement-level differences. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87 (3), 386–398. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.87.3.386 .

Shin, H., & Ryan, A. M. (2014). Friendship networks and achievement goals: An examination of selection and influence processes and variations by gender. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 43 (9), 1453–1464. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10964-014-0132-9 .

Simpkins, S. D., Fredricks, J. A., & Eccles, J. S. (2012). Charting the Eccles’ expectancy-value model from mothers’ beliefs in childhood to youths’ activities in adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 48 (4), 1019–1032. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027468 .

Simpson, R. D., & Oliver, J. (1990). A summary of major influences on attitude toward and achievement in science among adolescent students. Science Education, 74 (1), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.3730740102 .

Sjaastad, J. (2012). Sources of inspiration: The role of significant persons in young people’s choice of science in higher education. International Journal of Science Education, 34 (10), 1615–1636. https://doi.org/10.1080/09500693.2011.590543 .

Skinner, B. F. (1963). Operant behavior. American Psychologist, 18 (8), 503–515. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0045185 .

Skinner, E. A. (1995). Perceived control, motivation, and coping . Thousand Oaks: Sage.

Skinner, E. A., Chapman, M., & Baltes, P. B. (1988). Control, meansends, and agency beliefs: A new conceptualization and its measurement during childhood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 54 (1), 117–133. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.54.1.117 .

Skinner, E. A., Gembeck-Zimmer, M. J., & Connell, J. P. (1998). Individual differences and the development of perceived control. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 6 (2/3. Serial No. 254), 1–220.

Stipek, D., Recchia, S., McClintic, S., & Lewis, M. (1992). Self-evaluation in young children. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development , 57 (1. Serial No. 226) 1–97.

Swarat, S., Ortony, A., & Revelle, W. (2012). Activity matters: Understanding student interest in school science. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 49 (4), 515–537. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.21010 .

Tenenbaum, H. R., & Leaper, C. (2003). Parent-child conversations about science: The socialization of gender inequities? Developmental Psychology, 39 (1), 34–47. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.1.34 .

Thorndike, E. L. (1927). The law of effect. The American Journal of Psychology, 39 (1/4), 212–222. https://doi.org/10.2307/141541310.2307/1415413 .

Ushioda, E. (2007). Motivation, autonomy and sociocultural theory. In P. Benson (Ed.), Learner autonomy 8: Teacher and learner perspectives (pp. 5–24). Dublin: Authentik.

Vallerand, R. J. (1997). Toward a hierarchical model of intrinsic and extrinsic motivation. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 29 , 271–360. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60019-2 .

Vedder-Weiss, D., & Fortus, D. (2013). School, teacher, peers, and parents’ goals emphases and adolescents’ motivation to learn science in and out of school. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 50 (8), 952–988. https://doi.org/10.1002/tea.21103 .

Véronneau, M. H., Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., Dishion, T. J., & Tremblay, R. E. (2010). Transactional analysis of the reciprocal links between peer experiences and academic achievement from middle childhood to early adolescence. Developmental Psychology, 46 (4), 773. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019816 .

Volkow, N. D., Wang, G. J., Newcorn, J. H., Kollins, S. H., Wigal, T. L., Telang, F., & Wong, C. (2011). Motivation deficit in ADHD is associated with dysfunction of the dopamine reward pathway. Molecular Psychiatry, 16 (11), 1147–1154. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2010.97 .

Wang, M. T., & Eccles, J. S. (2012). Social support matters: Longitudinal effects of social support on three dimensions of school engagement from middle to high school. Child Development, 83 (3), 877–895. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01745.x .

Wang, M. T., & Sheikh-Khalil, S. (2014). Does parental involvement matter for student achievement and mental health in high school? Child Development, 85 (2), 610–625. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12153 .

Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92 (4), 548–573. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.92.4.548 .

Weiner, B., Frieze, I., Kukla, A., Reed, L., Rest, S., & Rosenbaum, R. M. (1987). Perceiving the causes of success and failure. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, & B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of behavior (pp. 95–120). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

Weisz, J. P. (1984). Contingency judgments and achievement behavior: Deciding what is controllable and when to try. In J. G. Nicholls (Ed.), The development of achievement motivation (pp. 107–136). Greenwich: JAI Press.

Wentzel, K. R. (1998). Social relationships and motivation in middle school: The role of parents, teachers, and peers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90 (2), 202–209. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.90.2.202 .

Wentzel, K. R. (2000). What is it that I’m trying to achieve? Classroom goals from a content perspective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25 (1), 105–115. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1999.1021 .

Wentzel, K. (2002). Are effective teachers like good parents? Teaching styles and student adjustment in early adolescence. Child Development, 73 (1), 287–301. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00406 .

Wentzel, K. R. (2005). Peer relationships, motivation, and academic performance at school. In C. S. Dweck & A. J. Elliot (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 279–296). New York: Guilford Press.

Wentzel, K. R., & Miele, D. B. (Eds.). (2009). Handbook of motivation at school . New York: Routledge.

Wentzel, K. R., & Muenks, K. (2016). Peer influence on students’ motivation, academic achievement, and social behavior. In K. R. Wentzel & G. B. Ramani (Eds.), Handbook of social influences in school contexts: Social-emotional, motivation, and cognitive outcomes (pp. 13–30). New York: Routledge.

Wentzel, K. R., Battle, A., Russell, S. L., & Looney, L. B. (2010). Social supports from teachers and peers as predictors of academic and social motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 35 (3), 193–202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2010.03.002 .

Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. (1992). The development of achievement task values: A theoretical analysis. Developmental Review, 12 (3), 265–310. https://doi.org/10.1016/0273-2297(92)90011-P .

Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2000). Expectancy–value theory of achievement motivation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25 (1), 68–81. https://doi.org/10.1006/ceps.1999.1015 .

Wigfield, A., & Eccles, J. S. (2002). The development of competence beliefs and values from childhood through adolescence. In A. Wigfield & J. S. Eccles (Eds.), Development of achievement motivation (pp. 92–120). San Diego: Academic Press.

Wigfield, A., & Tonks, S. (2004). The development of motivation for reading and how it is influenced by CORI. In J. T. Guthrie, A. Wigfield, & K. C. Perencevich (Eds.), Motivating reading comprehension: Concept-oriented reading instruction (pp. 249–272). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

Wigfield, A., & Wagner, A. L. (2005). Competence, motivation, and identity development during adolescence. In A. J. Elliot & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 222–239). New York: Guilford Press.

Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Yoon, K. S., Harold, R. D., Arbreton, A. J., Freedman-Doan, C., & Blumenfeld, P. C. (1997). Change in children’s competence beliefs and subjective task values across the elementary school years: A 3-year study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89 (3), 451. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89.3.451 .

Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Schiefele, U., Roeser, R. W., & Davis-Kean, P. (2006). Development of achievement motivation . In N. Eisenberg, W. Damon & R.M. Lerner (Vol. 3, pp.933–1002). Hoboken: Wiley. doi: https://doi.org/10.1002/9780470147658.chpsy0315 .

Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Schiefele, U., Roeser, R. W., & Davis‐Kean, P. (2007). Development of achievement motivation . Hoboken: Wiley.

Wigfield, A., Eccles, J. S., Roeser, R. W., & Schiefele, U. (2008). Development of achievement motivation. In W. Damon, R. M. Lerner, D. Kuhn, R. S. Siegler, & N. Eisenberg (Eds.), Child and adolescent development: An advanced course (pp. 406–434). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

Wlodkowski, R. J. (2011). Enhancing adult motivation to learn: A comprehensive guide for teaching all adults . San Francisco: Wiley.

Woodworth, R. S. (1918). Dynamic Psychology . New York: Columbia University Press.

Yeung, W. J., Linver, M. R., & Brooks–Gunn, J. (2002). How money matters for young children's development: Parental investment and family processes. Child Development, 73 (6), 1861–1879. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.t01-1-00511 .

Zimmerman, B. J., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1990). Student differences in self-regulated learning: Relating grade, sex, and giftedness to self-efficacy and strategy use. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82 (1), 51–59. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.82.1.51 .

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Escuela de Psicología, Universidad de los Andes, Santiago, Chile

Paulina Arango

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paulina Arango .

Editor information

Editors and affiliations.

School of Education, Universidad de los Andes, Las Condes, Chile

Pelusa Orellana García

Institute of Literature, Universidad de los Andes, Las Condes, Chile

Paula Baldwin Lind

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

Copyright information

© 2018 Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature

About this chapter

Arango, P. (2018). Motivation: Introduction to the Theory, Concepts, and Research. In: Orellana García, P., Baldwin Lind, P. (eds) Reading Achievement and Motivation in Boys and Girls. Literacy Studies, vol 15. Springer, Cham. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-75948-7_1

Download citation

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-75948-7_1

Published : 03 May 2018

Publisher Name : Springer, Cham

Print ISBN : 978-3-319-75947-0

Online ISBN : 978-3-319-75948-7

eBook Packages : Education Education (R0)

Share this chapter

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Publish with us

Policies and ethics

  • Find a journal
  • Track your research

Academic Coaching

  • Writing Resources
  • Free Report
  • Get a Quote

Tuesday 11 July 2017

Research proposal: motivation and background, motivation for your research.

what is motivation in a research paper

1.    Introduction

2.    background, 3.    research questions.

Stack Exchange Network

Stack Exchange network consists of 183 Q&A communities including Stack Overflow , the largest, most trusted online community for developers to learn, share their knowledge, and build their careers.

Q&A for work

Connect and share knowledge within a single location that is structured and easy to search.

Contribution vs Motivation

Can somebody explain to me what is the difference between contribution and motivation in any research paper?

  • publications
  • terminology

ff524's user avatar

Motivation : why is this an important problem? Why should anybody care about this?

For example:

X affects tens of thousands of consumers in the United States each year, and costs companies billions of dollars.
If we can solve X, we are one step closer to solving the biggest open problem in the field, Z.

Contribution : what is the new thing (result, approach, technique, whatever) that this paper describes, that advances the state of this field?

This paper describes a way to do X using Y, which is up to 200 times faster than the current state of the art.
  • Can we then say motivation describes the problem statement? –  Mike Commented Oct 23, 2015 at 0:01
  • 2 @user2999915 A problem statement could just be "Here is a problem X." That's not really motivation, unless you say "We really want to know how to do X because [insert motivation here]..." –  ff524 Commented Oct 23, 2015 at 0:03
  • @Mike Problem statement = "This is the problem we want to solve". Problem motivation = "The problem is relevant, because many people need to do X", contribution = "We solve these aspects of the problem". –  allo Commented May 26, 2021 at 16:15

You must log in to answer this question.

Not the answer you're looking for browse other questions tagged publications terminology ..

  • Featured on Meta
  • Bringing clarity to status tag usage on meta sites
  • We've made changes to our Terms of Service & Privacy Policy - July 2024
  • Announcing a change to the data-dump process

Hot Network Questions

  • When was this photo taken?
  • Op Amp Feedback Resistors
  • How can one says that a particle IS a representation of some group?
  • How to determine if a set is countable or uncountable?
  • Visualizing histogram of data on unit circle?
  • What does 北渐 mean?
  • Are chord inversions determined solely by the lowest note, even in a broken chord?
  • How should I respond to a former student from my old institution asking for a reference?
  • Can a "sharp turn" on a trace with an SMD resistor also present a risk of reflection?
  • Sticker on caption phone says that using the captions can be illegal. Why?
  • TeXbook Exercise 21.10 Answer
  • The hat-check problem
  • how replicate this effect with geometry nodes
  • Millennial reign and New Heaven and New Earth
  • "Knocking it out of the park" sports metaphor American English vs British English?
  • Can figere come with a dative?
  • Fitting 10 pieces of pizza in a box
  • Are Experimental Elixirs Magic Items?
  • Melee Opportunist--making opportunity attacks have some bite for melee characters
  • How to ensure a BSD licensed open source project is not closed in the future?
  • What Christian ideas are found in the New Testament that are not found in the Old Testament?
  • Calculate the sum of numbers in a rectangle
  • Fast circular buffer
  • Is the error in translation of Genesis 19:5 deliberate?

what is motivation in a research paper

  • Affiliate Program

Wordvice

  • UNITED STATES
  • 台灣 (TAIWAN)
  • TÜRKIYE (TURKEY)
  • Academic Editing Services
  • - Research Paper
  • - Journal Manuscript
  • - Dissertation
  • - College & University Assignments
  • Admissions Editing Services
  • - Application Essay
  • - Personal Statement
  • - Recommendation Letter
  • - Cover Letter
  • - CV/Resume
  • Business Editing Services
  • - Business Documents
  • - Report & Brochure
  • - Website & Blog
  • Writer Editing Services
  • - Script & Screenplay
  • Our Editors
  • Client Reviews
  • Editing & Proofreading Prices
  • Wordvice Points
  • Partner Discount
  • Plagiarism Checker
  • APA Citation Generator
  • MLA Citation Generator
  • Chicago Citation Generator
  • Vancouver Citation Generator
  • - APA Style
  • - MLA Style
  • - Chicago Style
  • - Vancouver Style
  • Writing & Editing Guide
  • Academic Resources
  • Admissions Resources

How to Write the Rationale of the Study in Research (Examples)

what is motivation in a research paper

What is the Rationale of the Study?

The rationale of the study is the justification for taking on a given study. It explains the reason the study was conducted or should be conducted. This means the study rationale should explain to the reader or examiner why the study is/was necessary. It is also sometimes called the “purpose” or “justification” of a study. While this is not difficult to grasp in itself, you might wonder how the rationale of the study is different from your research question or from the statement of the problem of your study, and how it fits into the rest of your thesis or research paper. 

The rationale of the study links the background of the study to your specific research question and justifies the need for the latter on the basis of the former. In brief, you first provide and discuss existing data on the topic, and then you tell the reader, based on the background evidence you just presented, where you identified gaps or issues and why you think it is important to address those. The problem statement, lastly, is the formulation of the specific research question you choose to investigate, following logically from your rationale, and the approach you are planning to use to do that.

Table of Contents:

How to write a rationale for a research paper , how do you justify the need for a research study.

  • Study Rationale Example: Where Does It Go In Your Paper?

The basis for writing a research rationale is preliminary data or a clear description of an observation. If you are doing basic/theoretical research, then a literature review will help you identify gaps in current knowledge. In applied/practical research, you base your rationale on an existing issue with a certain process (e.g., vaccine proof registration) or practice (e.g., patient treatment) that is well documented and needs to be addressed. By presenting the reader with earlier evidence or observations, you can (and have to) convince them that you are not just repeating what other people have already done or said and that your ideas are not coming out of thin air. 

Once you have explained where you are coming from, you should justify the need for doing additional research–this is essentially the rationale of your study. Finally, when you have convinced the reader of the purpose of your work, you can end your introduction section with the statement of the problem of your research that contains clear aims and objectives and also briefly describes (and justifies) your methodological approach. 

When is the Rationale for Research Written?

The author can present the study rationale both before and after the research is conducted. 

  • Before conducting research : The study rationale is a central component of the research proposal . It represents the plan of your work, constructed before the study is actually executed.
  • Once research has been conducted : After the study is completed, the rationale is presented in a research article or  PhD dissertation  to explain why you focused on this specific research question. When writing the study rationale for this purpose, the author should link the rationale of the research to the aims and outcomes of the study.

What to Include in the Study Rationale

Although every study rationale is different and discusses different specific elements of a study’s method or approach, there are some elements that should be included to write a good rationale. Make sure to touch on the following:

  • A summary of conclusions from your review of the relevant literature
  • What is currently unknown (gaps in knowledge)
  • Inconclusive or contested results  from previous studies on the same or similar topic
  • The necessity to improve or build on previous research, such as to improve methodology or utilize newer techniques and/or technologies

There are different types of limitations that you can use to justify the need for your study. In applied/practical research, the justification for investigating something is always that an existing process/practice has a problem or is not satisfactory. Let’s say, for example, that people in a certain country/city/community commonly complain about hospital care on weekends (not enough staff, not enough attention, no decisions being made), but you looked into it and realized that nobody ever investigated whether these perceived problems are actually based on objective shortages/non-availabilities of care or whether the lower numbers of patients who are treated during weekends are commensurate with the provided services.

In this case, “lack of data” is your justification for digging deeper into the problem. Or, if it is obvious that there is a shortage of staff and provided services on weekends, you could decide to investigate which of the usual procedures are skipped during weekends as a result and what the negative consequences are. 

In basic/theoretical research, lack of knowledge is of course a common and accepted justification for additional research—but make sure that it is not your only motivation. “Nobody has ever done this” is only a convincing reason for a study if you explain to the reader why you think we should know more about this specific phenomenon. If there is earlier research but you think it has limitations, then those can usually be classified into “methodological”, “contextual”, and “conceptual” limitations. To identify such limitations, you can ask specific questions and let those questions guide you when you explain to the reader why your study was necessary:

Methodological limitations

  • Did earlier studies try but failed to measure/identify a specific phenomenon?
  • Was earlier research based on incorrect conceptualizations of variables?
  • Were earlier studies based on questionable operationalizations of key concepts?
  • Did earlier studies use questionable or inappropriate research designs?

Contextual limitations

  • Have recent changes in the studied problem made previous studies irrelevant?
  • Are you studying a new/particular context that previous findings do not apply to?

Conceptual limitations

  • Do previous findings only make sense within a specific framework or ideology?

Study Rationale Examples

Let’s look at an example from one of our earlier articles on the statement of the problem to clarify how your rationale fits into your introduction section. This is a very short introduction for a practical research study on the challenges of online learning. Your introduction might be much longer (especially the context/background section), and this example does not contain any sources (which you will have to provide for all claims you make and all earlier studies you cite)—but please pay attention to how the background presentation , rationale, and problem statement blend into each other in a logical way so that the reader can follow and has no reason to question your motivation or the foundation of your research.

Background presentation

Since the beginning of the Covid pandemic, most educational institutions around the world have transitioned to a fully online study model, at least during peak times of infections and social distancing measures. This transition has not been easy and even two years into the pandemic, problems with online teaching and studying persist (reference needed) . 

While the increasing gap between those with access to technology and equipment and those without access has been determined to be one of the main challenges (reference needed) , others claim that online learning offers more opportunities for many students by breaking down barriers of location and distance (reference needed) .  

Rationale of the study

Since teachers and students cannot wait for circumstances to go back to normal, the measures that schools and universities have implemented during the last two years, their advantages and disadvantages, and the impact of those measures on students’ progress, satisfaction, and well-being need to be understood so that improvements can be made and demographics that have been left behind can receive the support they need as soon as possible.

Statement of the problem

To identify what changes in the learning environment were considered the most challenging and how those changes relate to a variety of student outcome measures, we conducted surveys and interviews among teachers and students at ten institutions of higher education in four different major cities, two in the US (New York and Chicago), one in South Korea (Seoul), and one in the UK (London). Responses were analyzed with a focus on different student demographics and how they might have been affected differently by the current situation.

How long is a study rationale?

In a research article bound for journal publication, your rationale should not be longer than a few sentences (no longer than one brief paragraph). A  dissertation or thesis  usually allows for a longer description; depending on the length and nature of your document, this could be up to a couple of paragraphs in length. A completely novel or unconventional approach might warrant a longer and more detailed justification than an approach that slightly deviates from well-established methods and approaches.

Consider Using Professional Academic Editing Services

Now that you know how to write the rationale of the study for a research proposal or paper, you should make use of Wordvice AI’s free AI Grammar Checker , or receive professional academic proofreading services from Wordvice, including research paper editing services and manuscript editing services to polish your submitted research documents.

You can also find many more articles, for example on writing the other parts of your research paper , on choosing a title , or on making sure you understand and adhere to the author instructions before you submit to a journal, on the Wordvice academic resources pages.

  • Skip to main content
  • Skip to primary sidebar
  • Skip to footer
  • QuestionPro

survey software icon

  • Solutions Industries Gaming Automotive Sports and events Education Government Travel & Hospitality Financial Services Healthcare Cannabis Technology Use Case AskWhy Communities Audience Contactless surveys Mobile LivePolls Member Experience GDPR Positive People Science 360 Feedback Surveys
  • Resources Blog eBooks Survey Templates Case Studies Training Help center

what is motivation in a research paper

Home Market Research

Motivational Research: Techniques, Strengths, and Weaknesses

Motivational research is a type of marketing research that attempts to understand why customers act the way they do. Find more about it here.

Motivational research is to identify and understand the aspects of the consumer that they do not fully comprehend. It makes the assumption that there are hidden or unconscious motivations influencing consumer behavior.

Motivational research gradually got linked with traditional marketing research, influencing and expanding the inventory of marketing research and research procedures. Its popularity peaked in the latter half of the 1950s and early 1960s.

So, in this blog, we will describe motivational research, its techniques, strengths, and weaknesses.

What is Motivational Research?

Motivational Research is the currently famous phrase for using psychiatric and psychological procedures to understand better why individuals react the way they do to products, advertisements, and other marketing scenarios.

Motivational research is consumer research that has grown in popularity in recent years. It is an endeavor to uncover and explain why customer behavior differs and why they behave in a specific manner.

Thus, it prompts the consumer’s inhibited (conscious) and repressed (unconscious) motives. In denial, the consumer is aware of his intentions but refuses to reveal them to others for fear of humiliation, punishment, or ostracism.

However, information regarding the motivating reason remains in the conscious mind. Because the individual will not accept the presence of a motive even to himself, repression suggests a more profound rejection of knowledge about an explanation.

LEARN ABOUT: Behavioral Research

Techniques of Motivational Research

Customers need to efficiently or accurately express their emotional feelings on direct questioning. As a result, Clinical Psychological methods/techniques are applied in motivational research for the psychoanalytical research of consumers.

All marketing activities require motivational research. The motivational study employs the following techniques:

01. Observation Technique

Consumer motivations can be deduced from their communication and actions. The researcher uses this strategy to observe consumer behavior without revealing their name. This procedure does not include any interviews.

Under this technique, the researcher may obtain relevant data or information individually using mechanical aids. This technique is appropriate for researching the purchasing process of consumers.

02. Depth Interview Technique

The researcher uses this strategy to conduct an intense interview.

The researcher questions respondents individually for several hours in the form of an interview. Interviewers are taught to develop rapport and not to steer the conversation too much. Respondents are urged to open up about their activities or interests and a specific issue or brand under investigation.

Interview transcripts are then thoroughly examined, along with comments on respondents’ moods and gestures they may have used to express attitudes or motives.

Such studies are appropriate for giving the market a head start on potential appeals.

03. Self Reports Technique

In this technique, the researcher merely verbally inquires about the respondent’s intentions, objectives, and needs or through a questionnaire.

The researcher may administer many tests using pencil and paper under this method of motivation study to elicit replies from the participants regarding their wants, desires, opinions, interests, reactions, etc.

The information is next quantified, which involves giving it a score to determine the degree to which a given need or motivation exists.

This technique has two measure restrictions:

  • First, people must be conscious of their actions’ causes or motivations.
  • Second, respondents might explain their words or actions. Respondents could be reluctant to disclose their genuine intentions and might be prone to providing socially acceptable answers.

04. Projective Techniques

Projective techniques aim to expose an individual’s genuine emotions and intentions.

Various cloaked tests with unclear stimuli, including unfinished sentences, cartoons, untitled images, other person characterization, etc., make up projective techniques .

Projective techniques operate on the fundamental presumption that participants are oblivious to the fact that they are expressing their emotions. These tactics need to give respondents a chance to explain their answers.

Some important projective techniques are as under:

  • Thematic Apperception Test (T.A.T)
  • Word Association
  • Sentence Completion
  • Third Person Technique
  • The Paired Pictures Test.

Strengths and weaknesses of motivational research

Like anything and everything else in this world, motivational research has some of its strengths and weaknesses. Some of them are given below:

Strength: 

Some of the strengths of motivational research include the following:

  • In-depth insights into consumer behavior and motivations
  • Assists marketers in tailoring their products and marketing messages better to fit the wants and wishes of their customers.
  • Can assist businesses in identifying unmet consumer requirements and product development prospects
  • Can give helpful information for market segmentation and target marketing
  • It can increase the success of marketing campaigns by identifying important motivators and drivers of consumer behavior.

Weaknesses:

Measuring the influence of motivational studies on consumer behavior and corporate outcomes can take time and effort. Some of the weaknesses of motivational research are as follows:

  • It can be time-consuming and costly, especially compared to other market research types.
  • It is based on self-reported data, which might be skewed and inaccurate.
  • It is often based on tiny, non-representative samples, which limits the conclusion’s generalizability.
  • It might be challenging to transform ideas from motivational research into tangible activities or marketing and product development initiatives.

LEARN ABOUT: Market research vs marketing research

Motivational research is a type of market research that tries to find out what drives, wants, and needs people really have. In-depth interviews, focus groups, and observation are all common ways to research what motivates people.

Overall, motivational research can be useful for businesses that want to learn more about their customers and make better marketing plans.

QuestionPro is survey software that can be used to research what motivates people. The platform has various features and tools that help researchers design, run, and analyze surveys for motivational research.

QuestionPro lets people create surveys with different kinds of questions, like multiple choice, open-ended, and rating scales. It also lets you change how surveys look and feel and target specific groups of respondents.

QuestionPro also has various tools for analyzing and reporting data that can help researchers make sense of the information they collect and come to useful conclusions. Overall, QuestionPro can be a useful tool for doing motivational research.

LEARN MORE         FREE TRIAL

MORE LIKE THIS

age gating

Age Gating: Effective Strategies for Online Content Control

Aug 23, 2024

what is motivation in a research paper

Customer Experience Lessons from 13,000 Feet — Tuesday CX Thoughts

Aug 20, 2024

insight

Insight: Definition & meaning, types and examples

Aug 19, 2024

employee loyalty

Employee Loyalty: Strategies for Long-Term Business Success 

Other categories.

  • Academic Research
  • Artificial Intelligence
  • Assessments
  • Brand Awareness
  • Case Studies
  • Communities
  • Consumer Insights
  • Customer effort score
  • Customer Engagement
  • Customer Experience
  • Customer Loyalty
  • Customer Research
  • Customer Satisfaction
  • Employee Benefits
  • Employee Engagement
  • Employee Retention
  • Friday Five
  • General Data Protection Regulation
  • Insights Hub
  • Life@QuestionPro
  • Market Research
  • Mobile diaries
  • Mobile Surveys
  • New Features
  • Online Communities
  • Question Types
  • Questionnaire
  • QuestionPro Products
  • Release Notes
  • Research Tools and Apps
  • Revenue at Risk
  • Survey Templates
  • Training Tips
  • Tuesday CX Thoughts (TCXT)
  • Uncategorized
  • What’s Coming Up
  • Workforce Intelligence

Academia.edu no longer supports Internet Explorer.

To browse Academia.edu and the wider internet faster and more securely, please take a few seconds to  upgrade your browser .

Enter the email address you signed up with and we'll email you a reset link.

  • We're Hiring!
  • Help Center

paper cover thumbnail

MOTIVATION: CONCEPT, THEORIES AND PRACTICAL IMPLICATIONS

Profile image of International Research Journal Commerce arts science

Management researchers have long believed that organisational goals are unattainable without effective use of motivational concepts. One key element of leadership is the ability to get others to do something, creating an influence. This paper is designed to help familiarise readers with the concepts related to motivation. Some popular theories will be reviewed and discussed. A leader must be able to motivate others to achieve goals, accomplish tasks, and complete objectives. Motivation is one key indicator of behaviour. We behave in a way that will satisfy motive based on a need. However, we don’t always have the luxury of knowing what our needs or motives are. Knowing potential areas of motivation, like the ones discussed in this module, can provide guidance in working with and discussing concerns with others.

Related Papers

MOHIUDDIN ZIA

what is motivation in a research paper

Khulna University Studies

Dr. Nazrul Islam

This paper examines the concepts, theories, and techniques of motivation, which are important aspects of managerial and administrative people in business and industry. The performance of any organization depends on the efficiency of individuals, and that could be achieved when they are motivated with their works. Motivation is the inner force or drive of individuals constituted through feelings, experience, realization of job, work, and behavior of management that directs them to accomplish their work assignments. Definitions pertaining to motivation also disclosed that this force is directed to satisfy or fulfill needs, wants, expectations, and desires This force could be made favorable by making change of needs and wants satisfying factors that are widely known as means of motivation. The motivational means might broadly be of two types financial and non-financial By adapting the means of motivation, company can create motivational and well-disciplined work environment that are essential for the present complex business world.

Academic Voices: A Multidisciplinary Journal

ganesh niraula

Motivation is the most important tool for modern organizations. Motivation actually crates drive that leads to the achievement of the individual as well as the organization’s objectives. Now a days many management experts have focused their studies in this field. Moreover not only business organizations, other types of organizations need motivation to obtain their desired goals. It is vital element in the field of education too, as we know very well that a teacher persuade and create drive the students to learn new things and lesson. The present study focuses on concept of motivation, importance of motivation and techniques of motivation at work places.Academic Voices, Vol. 3, No. 1, 2013, Pages 13-18 DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.3126/av.v3i1.9980

ali jabbari zahirabadi

One of the major responsibilities of managers in an organization is to identify potential capacities of the personnel and to bring about conditions for their growth and realization of these capacities which in turn, serves the basic goal of promoting productivity. In other words, gaining knowledge about motivational factors for improving performance and productivity of the organization is of great importance. Also, such this knowledge can greatly help to improve the application of human resources in organizations, prevent employees' resistance against change, reduce limitations in the output, and prevent conflict among personnel, a process which consequently results in a profitable organization. Motivation is about human life and realization of goals. Human beings live and work happily and hopefully when they feel alive and perceive a meaningful relationship between their work and life.

shehan samarasinghe

shwet goswami

Polonia University Scientific Journal

olena shtepa

Global Business Perspectives

Manoj Patwardhan

Jaleel Talaq

Organizational Effectiveness

John Arnold

Loading Preview

Sorry, preview is currently unavailable. You can download the paper by clicking the button above.

RELATED PAPERS

Sydney M Kabaso

Tanvir Hasan

MOTIVATING FACTORS

Javan Okwaro , ismael okwaro

Theories of Motivation: Identification, Analysis and Application

Ekow Lamptey

IAEME PUBLICATION

IAEME Publication

Constantin Coderie

Himanshu Pankaj Boro

Vibhor Mathur

IMPACT OF MOTIVATION ON EMPLOYEE PERFORMANCE IN AN ORGANISATION

Dr. Herbert Sekandi(Ph.D)

Business Management AS Tsenov Academy of Economics Journal of

Driton Sylqa

Buraimoh Gbolahan Michael

kansiime ronald

Soorej Parameeswaran

UKATA UCHNNA

jennifer Duru

Business Systems Research Journal

Petar Kurecic , Trina Mjeda

ashwini gorde

masego ntuane

Knowledge International Journal

Diturije Ismaili

Thanh Nguyen Minh

Samantha Davidson Fuller

CENTRAL ASIAN JOURNAL OF INNOVATIONS ON TOURISM MANAGEMENT AND FINANCE

Central Asian Studies

RELATED TOPICS

  •   We're Hiring!
  •   Help Center
  • Find new research papers in:
  • Health Sciences
  • Earth Sciences
  • Cognitive Science
  • Mathematics
  • Computer Science
  • Academia ©2024

Your Article Library

Motivation research: definition and techniques.

what is motivation in a research paper

ADVERTISEMENTS:

It is amply clear from the chapter CONSUMER BEHAVIOUR that the identification of consumer motives is a complex business. The difficulties involved in discovering directly the precise motivating factors that shape buying behaviour led some marketing psychologists to devise oblique techniques for exposing hidden motives.

For many years before World War II, researchers in the fields of psychology, psychiatry, sociology and social- anthropology had been conducting research into the behaviour of consumers, particularly probing of sub-conscious mind. Marketing research, today, can be conveniently divided into two parts ‘quantitative’ and ‘qualitative’.

The former concentrates on finding the answers to the questions: ‘What’ ‘Who’ ‘How many” Where’ and ‘When’ the consumer wants a product or service? On the other hand, qualitative research focuses its attention mainly on ‘Why he wants a product or service?

Put in other words, consumer ability to buy is not that important; what is more important are motives behind such buying.

In course of time, motivation research became integrated with traditional marketing research and influenced and expanded the inventory of marketing research and research techniques. It reached its high peak of popularity in the latter half of 1950s and early 1960s.

What is Motivation Research?

Motivation research is a form of consumer research which has gained ground over the recent years. Motivation Research is the currently popular term used to describe the application of psychiatric and psychological techniques to obtain a better understanding of why people respond as they do to products, ads and various other marketing situations.

It is an attempt to discover and explain why the consumer behaviour differs; why he or she behaves in a particular way?

What appeals and sales programmes will best influence his or her decision to act or buy or not to act or not to buy?

It concentrates on emotional or hidden stimuli to consumer action.

Thus, motivation research is an attempt to uncover the consumer’s suppressed (conscious) and repressed (unconscious) motives. In suppression, the consumer remains aware of his motives but does not care to admit their existence to others for the fear of ridicule, punishment or being ostracized.

Information about the motivating factor remains in the conscious mind, however. Repression implies a more serious rejection of knowledge about a motive because; the individual will not admit motive’s existence even to himself.

It is a careful probing beyond the surface “why” offered by consumers to explain their actions. As a branch of marketing research, it aims to discover the real reasons for their purchasing preferences via sample questionnaires, interviews and the like.

The theme of above definitions is that motivation research is to discover underlying motives, desires, instincts and emotions which provoke human behaviour and, in market research, true reasons why people buy or do not buy certain goods as distinct from the reasons they express or even imagine they act upon. The distinguishing feature of motivation research is probing “beneath the skin”, or “below the surface”.

Today, the most challenging task of marketing research is to predict how people will react and why they react in a particular way in a given situation. “How they react”? Can be answered with ease and confidence. Say, how the consumers receive the new product, package advertising message and the like where surveys cross tabulations and analysis can help to find the answers. However, more difficult task is one of finding out “why people” react in a particular way? By merely asking consumers why they like or dislike a product or an advertisement or a package, one cannot get satisfactory answers.

The answers differ widely and are misleading very often. These answers are misleading not because people are dishonest, but merely because they do not know really why? Wrong or unbelievable answers are given because of two possible reasons:

1. Conscious or unconscious attempt to rationalize their behaviour and

2. Preference for not disclosing their real reasons for ranking.

Conventional research does not answer this but motivation research does. It is the psycho-analysis that helps in overcoming the inability or the reluctance of people to tell why they like or dislike a product or a service. This is known as penetrating below the surface to reach sub-consciousness.

The relations between a consumer and a product are partly conscious and partly sub- conscious. The ‘price’ of a motor car, for example, is conscious relationship.

A person no doubt is worried and particular about kilometers per litre, cost per kilometer, boot-space acceleration and the like but his product preference of sub-conscious type is ‘sex symbol’.

Car is an extension of man’s personality which is known by motivation research. Precisely, motivation research is the art of finding out why? Without asking why?

The best example of understanding is one of choosing a girl as a house­wife. Ask a sober looking young man as to what type of woman he wants to marry; the answer may be “the quiet, home-loving more concerned about the food she cooks for me than the clothes, ornaments and make-up aids she wants”.

Observe him at parties and get-togethers and you will find that he is after a woman attractively clothed with gleaning make-up appealing to his hidden motives. Thus, motivation research is something that goes beneath the line. Thus, it is an attempt to market below the line.

Techniques of Motivation Research :

The techniques used in motivation research are of two types namely, Projective Techniques and Depth Interviews.

Projective Techniques :

These projective techniques represent the test conducted to establish the personalities of the respondents and their reactions to product media advertisement package product design and the like.

They project or reflect the subject’s thought about what he or she sees, feels, perceives thus producing the reactions.

These tests are derived from clinical psychology and work on the postulation that if an individual is placed in an ambiguous situation, he is guided by his own perceptions to describe the situation.

They often provide, an insight into the motives that lie below the level of consciousness and when the respondent is likely to rationalize his motives consciously or unconsciously; his responses tend to reflect his own attitudes and beliefs by indirection and discretion; they are his own perceptions and interpretations to the situation to which he is exposed.

There are five most commonly administered tests of this kind namely :

1. Thematic Appreciation Test.

2. Sentence Completion Test.

3. Word Association Test.

4. Paired Picture Test and

5. Third Person Test.

1. Thematic Appreciation Test (TAT):

Under this test, the respondent is presented with a picture or series of pictures of a scene or scenes involving people and objects associated with goods or services in questions. These are unstructured, doubtful in action and very often neutral giving no expression or motions. The respondent is to study the picture or the pictures and construct a story.

His narrations or readings are interpreted by a skilled analyst. Thus, the picture may be of a young man scribbling on a piece of a paper. Here, the respondent is to read as to whether the person in picture is writing. If so what? For whom? And why? And so on.

2. Sentence Completion Test (SCT):

Sentence completion tests are designed to discover emotional responses of the respondent. It is the easiest, most useful and reliable test to get the correct information in an indirect manner. The respondent is asked to complete the sentence given.

For instance, the questions may be, in case of ladies:

1. I like instant coffee because……………

2. I use talcum powder because…………..

3. I use electric kitchen gadgets because………………….

4. I do not use pain-killers like aspirin because……………..

5. I do not like red, brown and black colours because…………………….. In case of men, these questions may be

(a) I liked filter tipped cigarettes because………………….

(b) I gave up smoking because…………………

(c) I love natural proteins because………………..

(d) I prefer cold coffee because………………

(e) I do not use foam beds because……………….

The way the questions are asked, do not reflect right or wrong answers. However, the emotional values and tensions are reflected in the answers so given.

3. Word Association Test (WAT):

Word association test is similar to that of sentence completion test. The only difference is that instead of an incomplete sentence, a list of words ranging from twenty-five to seventy-five is given. This is the oldest and the simplest kind of test.

The respondent is to match the word. That is, the word suggested by the researcher is to be associated by the respondent by the most fitting word he thinks. This is widely used to measure the effect of the brand names and advertising messages.

Here, it is not possible to give all the seventy-five words. On illustrative basis, let us have fifteen words:

1. Perfume………..

2. Tooth paste

3. Hair oil………..

4. Shampoo……………………….

5. Shoes…………

6. Two-wheelers…………

7. Four-wheelers…………

8. Tyre………………………

9. Glass wares………….

10. Ink………..

11. Pencils…………

12. Fridges…………………….

13. Cupboards…………

14. Television………………….

15. Video cassettes………………

Thus, a respondent may give his preferences as ‘Colgate’ or ‘Promise’ or ‘Close-up’ or ‘Forhans’ in case of tooth paste. On the basis of such answers, it is possible to determine a scale of preference.

4. Paired Picture Test (PPT):

This is another very appealing and easy to administer test. Paired picture test means that the respondent is given a pair of pictures almost identical in all respects except in one. For instance, the researcher may be interested in knowing the reaction of respondents to a new brand of refrigerator.

The pair of pictures may show a woman opening refrigerator which is moderately priced with a usual brand; another picture of the same woman opening the refrigerator door of another brand.

Looking to these two pictures, the respondent is to give his own feelings or reactions. Though the same pair is shown to so many respondents, the reactions differ from person to person. Instead of using the usual figures, cartoons may be introduced. The analyst gets here the inner feelings of an individual for this analysis purpose.

5. Third Person Test (TPT):

The format of this test is that the respondent is given a photograph of a third person—may be a friend, a colleague, a neighbour, a star, a player, a professional and the like The point involved is that the researcher is interested in knowing what the third person thinks of an issue as heard through the respondent.

It is assumed that the respondent’s answer will reveal his own inner feelings more clearly through the third person than otherwise it would have been possible.

The best example of this kind the test conducted on American house-wives in connection with ‘instant coffee’. Prior to the test, the attitude of house-wives was “It does not taste good”, with the test being conducted, the real attitude was “A lady using instant coffee is lazy, spend-thrift and not a good house­wife”. This amply clears the fact how the test revealed the naked truth.

In addition to these tests of usual type, the researchers do use other qualitative techniques such as role-playing, psycho-drama, graphology and the like.

Related Articles:

  • Motivation Research: Strengths and Weaknesses
  • Different Studies of Marketing Research

Comments are closed.

web statistics

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Open access
  • Published: 23 August 2024

Different motivation, different achievements: the relationship of motivation and dedication to academic pursuits with final grades among Jewish and Arab undergraduates studying together

  • Efrat Gill 1   na1 ,
  • Oz Guterman 1   na1 &
  • Ari Neuman   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-1416-5522 2   na1  

Humanities and Social Sciences Communications volume  11 , Article number:  1079 ( 2024 ) Cite this article

Metrics details

Much research has indicated disparities between majority and minority groups in academic achievements. In Israel, differences have been recorded between the ethnic majority of students of Jewish origin and the ethnic minority of students of Arab origin. One possible reason for these findings might be differences in motivation, influenced by the respective cultures of the Jewish ethnic majority and Arab ethnic minority. The present research examined the relationship between differences in academic achievements of 73 students of Jewish origin and 74 students of Arab origin studying together and patterns of motivation and dedication to academic pursuits. The findings indicate considerable differences between the two populations in final grades and in motivational patterns and dedication to academic pursuits. In addition, in each of the research populations, different motivations were associated with a higher level of grades.

Similar content being viewed by others

what is motivation in a research paper

Race, academic achievement and the issue of inequitable motivational payoff

what is motivation in a research paper

Self-perceptions as mechanisms of achievement inequality: evidence across 70 countries

what is motivation in a research paper

Black adolescents’ motivation to resist the false dichotomy between mathematics achievement and racial identity

Academic achievement among majority and minority groups.

In many societies, academic education is considered key to social mobility and is especially important for minority groups (Alsulami, 2018 ; Baskakova et al., 2017 ; Bautista et al., 2023 ; Dominguez-Whitehead, 2017 ). In a review of studies that examined the effect of academic education on the social status of minority groups, Arar and Mustafa ( 2011 ) found that academic education strengthened minority groups by enabling better integration into the labor market, thereby enabling the minority groups better economic, social, and political status. Given the importance of this factor, it is alarming to see data on the gaps between the majority and minority groups in academic achievement. This gap does not exist in all minority groups and there are even cases in which minority groups have higher academic achievements. However, in cases where there is a gap, it is an obstacle to the progress of minority groups (Pérez-Martín and Villardón-Gallego, 2023 ).

Gaps in education and academic success between majority and minority groups at all levels of education have been widely documented in the literature. According to Martin et al. ( 2017 ), this is one of the most difficult and frustrating problems for policymakers. For example, the data on the gaps between students of African American and of white origin in the United States over the last three decades indicated significant differences in grades in favor of the latter (Bowen and Bok, 1998 ; Charles et al., 2009 ; Hung et al., 2020 ). At the college level, students of Latin American and African American origin in the United States had lower academic achievements (average grade and graduation scores) and took a longer time to graduate compared with students of white or Asian origin (Bowen et al., 2009 ; Kugelmass and Ready, 2011 ).

The gap between academic achievements of majority and minority groups in Israel

In Israel, too, there are gaps between the ethnic majority and minority groups, such as, for example, students of Jewish and Arab origin. Arar and Mustafa ( 2011 ) contended that the characteristics of students of Arab origin in Israel’s higher education system were similar to those of other minority groups in the world.

Lufi and Parish-Plass ( 2010 ) argued that the differences between Jewish-origin and Arab-origin students in academic achievements were reflected in a number of academic and educational indicators. Feniger et al. ( 2013 ) found that after completing high school, about 69% of Jewish-origin students eligible for a matriculation certificate continued on to higher education, compared with only 51% of Arab-origin students. Moreover, 74% of the Jewish-origin students completed their studies at the end of the qualifying period, compared with 62% of the Arab-origin students. In other words, about half (51%) of the Jewish-origin students entitled to a matriculation certificate completed a bachelor’s degree, compared with about one-third (32%) of the Arab-origin students. In addition to their relatively low rates of enrollment in academic institutions, the dropout rate among Arab-origin students has been shown to be higher, approximately 16.6% within two years of entering, compared with 12% among Jewish-origin students (Mustafa, 2007 ). Examination of the data on gaps in education over several years revealed an upward trend in the level of education among the Arab population in the last decades, however, the gap between Jews and Arabs remained (Guterman and Gill, 2023 ; Central Bureau of Statistics, 2020 ). These differences were also found to be associated with cultural disparities. The Arab society in Israel is characterized by a higher level of collectivism compared to Jewish society, and these differences were found to be linked to the academic achievements of students, among other factors. (Guterman et al., 2024a ). The Arab society in Israel is generally more traditional, placing a greater emphasis on belonging to the extended family. In this regard, family identity is, on average, more significant for Arab-origin students, and there is a tendency in Arab society in Israel towards more collectivist motivations (Guterman et al., 2024b ).

Differences in motivation and dedication to academic pursuits and cultural differences between Jews and Arabs in Israel

One of the explanations for these gaps between students of Jewish and Arab origin in academic achievements may be motivational differences. Several studies examined the level of motivation of high-school students of Jewish and Arab origin and showed that the desire to pursue academic degrees was higher among students of Arab origin (e.g., Feniger et al., 2021 ; Khattab, 2005 ). However, the effect of different types of motivation on the academic success of the two groups was not examined.

One of the possible explanations for the differences between students of Jewish and Arab origin in Israel in level of motivation and dedication to academic pursuits lies in the cultural differences between these population groups in terms of collectivism and individualism. As explained, previous research has found that students of Arab origin have a higher tendency toward collectivism than students of Jewish origin do (e.g., Amzaleg and Masry-Herzallah, 2022 ).

Research on motivation and dedication to academic pursuits

Several academic models and theories are dedicated to examining the motivation and dedication that individuals exhibit toward their academic endeavors. Each of these theoretical frameworks offers different explanations for the gap in academic achievements between Jewish and Arab populations in Israel, as well as potential disparities between these two groups in motivation and dedication to academic pursuits. Achievement goal theory (AGT; Ames, 1992 ), one of the most widely used theoretical frameworks in the research of motivation to learn (Huang, 2012 ; Urdan and Kaplan, 2020 ), explains students’ purposes for studying. Elliot and Trash ( 2001 ) defined motivation for achievement, and especially motivation for academic achievement as the purpose for which a person engages in goal-driven activities (p. 140). A number of studies have shown the connection between achievement goals in an academic setting and students’ learning behaviors (see, e.g., Meece et al., 2006 ) and examined the characteristics of students who continue to succeed in challenging learning environments.

Earlier studies in the field distinguished between two types of goals: mastery goals, in which the purpose is to develop abilities, and performance goals, in which the purpose is to demonstrate mastery (Ames, 1992 ; Dweck and Leggett, 1988 ). Recent studies have added an approach-avoidance dimension, whereby approach goals arise from the motivation to succeed or the motivation to avoid failure, respectively. Huang ( 2012 ) argued that approach motivation is generally associated with higher academic achievement and avoidance motivation is associated with lower academic achievement.

The underlying factor of motivational orientations is the way in which students assign meaning to school and learning. Nicholls ( 1992 ) argues that mastery goals and performance goals stem from students’ theories about education and learning. These theories are shaped by cultural meanings associated with education in their respective communities and their personal experiences within educational contexts (Maehr and Nicholls, 1980 ). This perspective suggests that in schools within different cultural groups, students may have different theories regarding school and learning.

Significant cultural differences are evident between minority and majority groups. Generally, majority groups adhere to a distinct cultural group that is different from that of minority groups, allowing for the distinction between ‘majority culture’ and ‘minority culture.' Indeed, this viewpoint aligns with the findings that highlight disparities between these groups, in academic achievements. For example, the 2009 Program for International Student Assessment (PISA), identified significant achievement gaps between immigrant and non-immigrant students was identified across all domains in most OECD countries. Immigrant students generally exhibited lower proficiency levels compared to their non-immigrant counterparts: in reading in 23 countries, in mathematics in 24 countries, and in science in 25 out of 28 countries. Additionally, academic difficulties were more prevalent among second-generation immigrants compared to first-generation immigrants (OECD, 2012 ).

Disparities in academic achievements were found in Israel between the majority Jewish population and the Arab minority. The gap in achievements was evident in the results of the 2002 PISA exam. According to the exam results, students of Arab origin achieved lower scores than those of Jewish origin, ranking Israel 31st in mathematics, 30th in reading, and 33rd in science out of 42 countries assessed. Conversely, if only scores of Jewish-origin students were considered, Israel would have ranked 12th. It was found that ~60% of students of Arab origin in Israel struggle with reading comprehension, compared to 30% among students of Jewish origin. In the Arab sector, despite an improvement in academic achievements, the eligibility rates for matriculation certificates remain low; among students of Arab origin, the eligibility rate stood at 34%, compared to 51% among students of Jewish origin. Regarding the quality of the matriculation certificate, a higher percentage of Jewish-origin students met the university entry requirements—87%, compared to 73% among Arab-origin students. Additionally, the dropout rate among youth is significantly higher in the Arab sector (Mi-Ami, 2003 ).

Indeed, previous research on differences between Jewish and Arab students in terms of achievements indicated the important role of learning goals (Guterman et al., 2024a ). The study found that the level of approach-avoidance goals of the Arab students was indeed lower than those of the Jewish students. This finding suggests the possibility that the collectivist perceptions of this population might lead to less willingness to create challenges, thus creating more passive and less active coping.

Another common model of motivation is the expectancy-value theory (EVT), a motivational framework that describes the correlation between an individual’s expectation of success in a task and the perceived value attributed to that task. First introduced by Atkinson in 1957 EVT was further developed by Wigfield and Eccles ( 2000 ). This theory comprises two principal components: expectancy and value. Expectancy is an individual’s belief in their ability to achieve success in a task, addressing the question, “Can I effectively execute this task?” Expectancy beliefs are influenced by past accomplishments or failures, thus shaping one’s perception of their likelihood of success. Value refers to the perceived importance, utility, or enjoyment associated with a task, addressing the question, “Do I consider this task worthwhile?” Value is influenced by an individual’s prior experiences, beliefs, and personal objectives. EVT identifies four distinct types of values: intrinsic value (the enjoyment derived from the task), attainment value (the personal significance of achieving success in the task), utility value (the practical usefulness of the task), and cost (the negative aspects associated with engagement in the task).

EVT offers an interesting look at the differences between groups. However, unlike AGT, differences between Jews and Arabs have not yet been examined from the perspective of this theory. In our opinion, and in accordance with EVT, there are no expected differences between the groups in the degree of desire to succeed (value). In other words, even though it is possible that the desire to succeed stems from different motivations, such as a desire for personal achievement (individualism) or a desire for group achievements (collectivism), there is no reason to assume that the desire for success itself would be different.

In contrast, there may be differences in the belief of individuals in their ability to succeed (expectancy). An individualistic perception directs individuals to focus on their own abilities. Indeed, research has shown a correlation between individualism and self-efficacy (Earley, 1994 ). In this respect, according to EVT, students of Jewish origin, who come from a more individualistic society, can be expected to show more active strategies compared with students of Arab origin, whose society is more collectivist.

The present study

The present study focused on motivation and dedication to academic pursuits among students of Jewish and Arab origin who were studying together in a college in Israel. To this end, a questionnaire validated in previous research in Israel was used to examine academic motivational patterns (Eliassy, 1999 ). The students were enrolled at an institution in which approximately half of the students were Jews and the other half were Arabs. The research examined several hypotheses:

In keeping with previous research, including studies conducted with students in academic institutions in Israel (Guterman and Neuman, 2019 ), a gap will be found between the groups in student grades, where the students of Jewish origin will have higher final grades than their peers of Arab origin.

A positive correlation will be found between the level of motivation to study and the final grade point average.

In light of the character of Arab culture in Israel, which is typically more collectivist than Jewish culture (Lapidot-Lefler and Hosri, 2016 ; Sagy et al., 2001 ), the level of passive engagement in learning (which refers to the performance of assignments given by lecturers) will be higher among students of Arab origin compared with those of Jewish origin. As noted, this hypothesis is based on previous findings that suggested a greater reliance on sources of authority as a basis for personal action in Arabs compared with Jewish society in Israel (Lapidot-Lefler and Hosri, 2016 ; Sagy et al., 2001 ).

Participants

The research was conducted with 147 students, all enrolled at the same college in Israel. The sample was divided between 74 students of Arab origin and 73 students of Jewish origin (according to their self-reports, as explained later). To enable examination of differences between the groups, we matched them in terms of the gender and age of the participants.

At the time of the research, the students were in their second year of bachelor’s degree studies. There were 109 female students (74.15% of the sample) and 38 male students (25.85%). Differences between the samples in the distribution of genders were examined using Chi-square analysis; no significant differences were found between the groups of Arab and Jewish origin in terms of the distribution of men and women; Χ 2 (1) = 0.96, p  > 0.5 (among the Arab students: 55 women and 19 men; among the Jewish students: 54 women and 19 men).

The participants’ ages ranged from 18 to 57. The mean age was 24.78 with a standard deviation of 6.64. To examine the differences between the groups in this respect, an independent sample t -test was conducted. No significant differences were found, t (145) = 0.44, p  > 0.05 (Arabs: M  = 24.54, SD = 7.27; Jews: M  = 25.02, SD = 5.98).

The researchers invited social sciences students to participate in the research during their classes at Western Galilee College. They explained that participation was voluntary and were assured that the data would not affect their grades or be used for any purpose other than the research. After the students gave their consent to participate in the research, meetings were arranged to administer the different questionnaires included in the research. The students signed to indicate their consent to participate in the research as well as permission for the research team to examine their final grades. The grades were collected two and a half years after completion of the questionnaires, according to the approval of the Ethics Committee of the college. Seven students dropped out of studies and were therefore not included in the research.

Instruments

Motivational patterns questionnaire.

Eliassy ( 1999 ) developed a questionnaire of 24 items in which the respondents rank the degree to which the statements fit them on a scale of 1 (“not at all true”) to 4 (“true to a great degree”). The items refer to different aspects of studying that represent patterns of high and low motivation in terms of quality and test the degree to which the student expresses willingness to demonstrate each of them. The Hebrew version regarding motivational patterns includes five subscales: (a) persistence when encountering difficulties during studies (such as difficult questions in homework assignments, study material that is hard to understand, and the like). In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha for this subscale was 0.71; (b) active involvement in studying, where the items examine the degree to which the student demonstrates interest in what is going on in class during lessons and their active participation in activities during lessons (that is, the degree to which the student engages in actions that reflect involvement, such as expressing an opinion, raising one’s hand, and the like). In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha for this subscale was 0.84; (c) passive involvement in studying, where the items also examine the degree of interest expressed by the student in what is happening in class during lessons, but in this case referring to situations when they turn their attention to what is happening during the lesson but do not take any specific action. In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha for this subscale was 0.82; (d) willingness to invest effort in studying (the degree to which the student is willing or chooses to put effort and time into studies, both in class and at home). In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha for this subscale was 0.81; and (e) seeking challenges in studying (the extent to which the student prefers to engage in complex or simple assignments in studies, in terms of the level of difficulty and the personal challenge they pose to the individual). In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha for this subscale was 0.77.

Academic achievements

The student’s final grades were collected from the college’s databases. The data were collected in accordance with the consent of the study participants and the approval of the Research Ethics Committee.

Demographic questionnaire

The respondents completed a demographic questionnaire that included questions about gender, study track, year of studies, age, and ethnic origin.

To test the hypotheses, several stages of analysis were conducted. First, the differences between Jewish and Arab students in all variables were analyzed. Second, the relationships between the research variables were examined, and finally, hierarchical regression was conducted to examine interactions between the variables in their contribution to explaining the variance in the final bachelor’s degree grades.

Differences between the students of Arab and Jewish origin in terms of the variables

To examine the differences between the students of Jewish origin and those of Arab origin in motivational patterns, a one-way MANOVA was performed. To examine the differences between the groups in final grades, a t -test was conducted.

The MANOVA regarding differences between the students of Jewish and Arab origin in motivational patterns showed a significant difference between the groups, F (5,141) = 4.01, p  < /−1. Eta 2  = 0.13. The results, means, and standard deviations of the motivational patterns by group are presented in Table 1 .

As the table shows, significant differences were found between the students of Jewish origin and their classmates of Arab origin in motivational patterns of willingness to invest effort in studies and seeking challenges in studies. In both these variables, the scores of the students of Jewish origin were higher, on average, than those of the students of Arab origin.

In addition, an independent sample t -test was conducted to examine whether there were differences between the students of Jewish and Arab origin in their final grades; it showed significant differences between the students in final grades, t (206) = 6.53, p  < 0.001. The average grades of the students of Jewish origin were higher than those of the students of Arab origin (Arabs: M  = 71.98, SD = 9.91; Jews: M  = 81.93, SD = 8.51).

Correlations among the research variables

To examine the correlations among the research variables and the between them and the final grades, Pearson correlations were calculated for each group. The correlations between the two motivational patterns and between these patterns and the final grade among students of Jewish origin and of Arab origin are presented in Tables 2 and 3 .

The tables show that among the students of Arab origin, there was a positive correlation between the final grade and passive involvement in studies. In comparison, among the students of Jewish origin, there was a positive correlation between final grade and active involvement in studies. In other words, among the students of Arab origin, the greater their passive involvement in studies was, the higher their final grades were, and among the students of Jewish origin, the greater their active involvement in studies, the higher their final grades were.

In contrast, in both groups, a negative correlation was found between involvement in studies (whether active or passive) and persistence when encountering difficulties with studies. In other words, in both groups, the less the student’s passive or active involvement in studies, the less their persistence when facing difficulty. In fact, in both groups, there was a positive correlation between the two types of involvement in studies (passive and active), where the greater the involvement of one type was, the greater the involvement of the other, as well; in other words, these variables were not independent. Furthermore, in both groups, a negative correlation was found between willingness to invest effort in studies and persistence when encountering academic difficulties. In other words, the greater the willingness to invest effort in studies, the lower the level of persistence when encountering difficulty. In both groups, a positive correlation was found between persistence when encountering academic difficulties and seeking challenges in studies: the greater the persistence when encountering difficulties, the greater, too, was the student’s search for challenges when studying.

In both groups, there was a positive correlation between passive involvement in studies and willingness to invest effort in studies. However, among the students of Jewish origin, there was also a positive correlation between active involvement and willingness to invest effort in studies, but among those of Arab origin such a correlation was not found. In other words, in both groups, the greater the passive involvement in studies, the greater the willingness to invest effort in studies, but only among those of Jewish origin did we also find that the greater the active involvement in studies, the greater the willingness to invest effort in studies.

In both groups, a negative correlation was found between passive involvement in studies and seeking challenges in studies, that is, the greater the passive involvement in studies, the less the search for challenges. Furthermore, among the students of Jewish origin, a negative correlation was also found between passive involvement in studies and willingness to invest effort in studies, that is, the greater the passive involvement in studies, the lower the willingness to invest effort. However, among the studies of Arab origin, no correlation was found between these two variables.

Regression analysis of explained variance of final grades among students of Jewish and Arab origin

To examine the contribution of the research variables to the explained variance in final grades, a separate regression analysis was performed for each of the groups. In both analyses, the first stage was to perform a multiple regression that included all the variables mentioned, even though some of them were not found to be associated with the final grade. The purpose of this analysis was to examine whether these variables might be found to contribute due to interaction with other variables. Next, a hierarchical regression was performed, where the variables that had been shown to correlate with the final grade, either as a main effect or an interaction, were entered. These regressions included three steps: (a) demographic characteristics (age and gender); (b) motivational patterns (persistence when encountering difficulties in studies, active involvement in studies, passive involvement in studies, willingness to invest effort in studies, and seeking challenges in studies); and (c) interaction of the motivational patterns with the demographic characteristics, to examine whether the contribution of the motivational patterns was dependent on the demographic characteristics of the student.

In the first two steps, the variables were force-entered; in the third step, which examined the contribution of the interactions to the explained variance, only those interactions that had been found to contribute to the explained variance significantly ( p  < 0.05) were entered. The regression regarding the students of Arab origin indicated that the level of explained variance was 30%; in comparison, the regression regarding students of Jewish origin indicated that the level of explained variance was 13%. The beta coefficients of the explained variance in each of the regressions are presented in Table 4 .

As the table shows, in the regression regarding the students of Arab origin, the results of the first step, which included only the demographic variables (age and gender), showed a significant contribution of 14% to the explained variance in final grades. In the regression regarding the students of Jewish origin, the same regression did not indicate a contribution to the explained variance in final grades. In the regression regarding the students of Arab origin, age was found to correlate positively with final grade: the older the student, the higher the grade. In the second step, when the five variables of the student’s motivational patterns (persistence when encountering difficulties in studies, active involvement in studies, passive involvement in studies, willingness to invest effort in studies, and seeking challenges in studies) were entered, both the regression regarding the students of Arab origin and that regarding students of Jewish origin indicated a significant contribution of 12% to the explained variance.

In the regression regarding students of Jewish origin, active involvement in studies correlated positively with the final grade (the greater the active involvement in studies, the higher the final grade). Furthermore, in the regression regarding Arab students, passive involvement in studies correlated positively with final grade (the greater the passive involvement in studies, the higher the final grade). In addition, among the students of Arab origin, a negative correlation was found between active involvement in studies and final grade (the greater the active involvement, the lower the final grade).

In the third step, when the interaction of willingness to invest effort in studies with age was entered, a significant contribution was found in the group of students of Arab origin. This interaction contributed an additional 4% to the explained variance in final grades among the Arab students. In the regression regarding students of Jewish origin, this interaction did not contribute significantly to explained variance in grades.

To reach a deeper understanding of the interactions, Aiken and West’s ( 1991 ) method was employed. Figure 1 presents a graphic description of the interaction of “willingness to invest effort in studies” with “age” among the students of Arab origin.

figure 1

Relationship between willingness to invest effort in studies and final grades among older and younger students.

As shown, among the younger students of Arab origin, the willingness to invest effort in studies did not correlate significantly with final grades, β  = 0.20, p  > 0.5. In contrast, among the more mature students of Arab origin, a significant negative correlation was found between willingness to invest effort in studies and final grades, β  = –0.23, p  < 0.5. In other words, among these students, the greater their willingness to invest effort in studies, the lower their final grades were.

Consistent with the research hypotheses and the findings of previous research, a significant difference was found between the groups of students in final undergraduate grades. The students from the Jewish ethnic majority had higher grades than those from the Arab ethnic minority. In addition to this finding, which corroborates that of earlier studies, the results of the present study also indicated differences between the groups in terms of motivational patterns. The students of Jewish origin scored higher than those of Arab origin in seeking challenges and willingness to invest effort in learning.

These findings are consistent with the two models presented in the introduction with respect to seeking challenges. One possible explanation arises from the difference between the groups in achievements. The grades of the students of Arab origin were lower; therefore, it is reasonable that the studies were more difficult for them, and this may have posed a greater threat to them compared with their Jewish counterparts. As presented in the introduction, this finding is consistent with many research findings on the gap between Jews and Arabs in Israel in academic achievements (Ayalon et al., 2019 ; Blass, 2020 ; TIMSS, 2023 ; OECD, 2018 ; Zuzovsky, 2008 ).

These figures are not unique to Israel. Modern society is characterized by substantial migration both between and within countries, leading to the intersection of diverse languages, cultures, and identities. Consequently, numerous nations are comprised of different ethnic minorities that are distinguished by distinctive characteristics. This situation, occasionally intensified by successive waves of migration, gives rise to numerous social advantages alongside complex challenges. Among the latter, the socioeconomic assimilation of minority group members stands out prominently. Discrepancies between majority and minority population groups in educational accomplishments often serve as a notable contributor to the prevailing disparities in these realms (Lauri et al., 2022 ; OECD, 2019 ; U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics, 2023 ).

In light of the greater difficulty, they may have been less able to seek challenges in their studies. In further research, it would be interesting to examine the level of threat that students experience and the relationship between this and the degree to which they seek challenges.

With regard to the willingness to invest effort in studies, the results were contrary to the research hypotheses. It was hypothesized that the need to cope with a greater academic challenge would lead to a greater willingness to invest in studies. However, the more complex task of coping for the Arab students may have created an opposite effect, that is, perhaps it led them to give up and created a negative feeling, expressed in less willingness to invest effort in studies. Another possible explanation of this finding might be based on the cultural differences between the groups. As discussed in the Introduction (Lapidot-Lefler and Hosri, 2016 ; Sagy et al., 2001 ), Arab society in Israel is more collectivist and places greater emphasis on authority figures than Jewish society does. Accordingly, it is possible that the students of Arab origin tended to invest more according to the social demands led by the authority figures of the lecturers, and were therefore less inclined to invest beyond the formal requirements. In further research, it would be interesting to examine these explanations by means of qualitative interviews with students from both sectors, which could shed light on the feelings created by facing difficulties and its impact on students’ willingness to invest effort in studies, as well as the way members of the two groups perceive the concept of investing in studies.

Another finding that emerged from the research was the positive correlation among the students of Arab origin between passive engagement in learning and final grades, compared to the positive correlation among the Jewish students between active engagement in learning and final grades. Possible explanations for this finding might also be drawn from the results of previous research that compared these two groups. Specifically, here too, the finding may stem from the different attitudes of the students of Jewish and Arab origin to authority figures. Students from the more traditional Arab society, which places greater emphasis on authority, might be less inclined to be active and take the initiative beyond the specific definitions of the system so that their main effort is expressed in passive learning. In contrast, students from less traditional Jewish society, which stresses authority less, may tend to base their efforts more on personal initiative and less on the demands of the system. It would be interesting for further research to include measures associated with cultural variables, such as conformism or the perception of authority figures, in order to examine these explanations.

Another finding of the present study was the lack of correlation between willingness to invest in studies and final grades among the younger students of Arab origin, and the negative correlation between these two variables among the older students of Arab origin. This might indicate a change in perceptions over time. As presented earlier, among students of Jewish origin, there was a positive correlation between willingness to invest effort in studies and final grades. One possible explanation of these findings is that these students, who were raised in modern Arab society, are less traditional and less influenced by the potential conflict between a collectivist society that places more emphasis on instructions of the system and less on personal effort and the competitive, individualistic society in which they study. This might explain the negative correlation found among the older, but not the younger students of Arab origin. In this case, too, further research that combines measures of the level of collectivism of students might be useful in examining this explanation of the present findings.

The findings of the present research are important, but the study nevertheless had some limitations. First, the types of motivation considered were derived from the literature and research instruments. There is an advantage to using instruments that have been tested in previous research as well as previous research findings, but this method also has an intrinsic shortcoming. For example, the present study did not include an examination of the fit of the division into types of motivation presented in the questionnaires with the taxonomy of types of motivation as perceived by these participants. In the future, it would be interesting to conduct a qualitative examination of how students perceive the types of motivation they feel, followed by a quantitative examination of the research questions of the present study, taking this taxonomy into account.

Second, because of the limited number of respondents, the research examined differences between the groups without considering their subgroups, which could have a strong impact on the findings. For example, the Arab ethnic minority in Israel is composed of several different groups, such as Muslims, Christians, and Druze, which have different characteristics. The Jewish population in Israel can also be divided into groups in different ways. It would be interesting for further research to examine the impact of belonging to these groups on the attitudes and perceptions of the respondents in the two groups. Third, the present research referred to undergraduate studies in a mixed institution, where about half of the respondents belonged to each group. Naturally, the results might be different in other institutions, with different proportions between the groups. It is essential to continue to study this important issue among different population groups, in order to obtain a fuller view of the findings.

Despite these limitations, the present research offers an important contribution. It deepens the understanding of the relationship between the types of motivation and the academic achievements of students. Moreover, the results indicated how this relationship might be influenced by the cultural and personal characteristics of the learners, thus providing a new perspective and enhancing the understanding of this field. Understanding differences between ethnic minority and majority groups also contributes, together with further research in the field, to the ability to help members of ethnic minorities integrate and advance socially, by means of evidence-based practice. From this perspective, research in this field may inform better-focused and well-founded assistance to groups that currently demonstrate lower achievements. Thus, it is hoped that the present research will serve as the basis for further studies that will broaden the understanding of this field.

For instance, targeted interventions aimed at low socioeconomic status (SES) populations can concentrate on defining diverse goals and ensuring alignment with these objectives. Such research illustrates that the challenge lies not only in skills but also in addressing a cultural gap concerning learning objectives. Given that learning goals correlate with academic achievements, efforts to uplift low-SES populations should emphasize not only technical aspects but also the perception of learning goals. Moreover, understanding the correlation between culture and learning goals paves the way for a more profound theoretical comprehension of this domain.

Aiken LS, West SG (1991) Multiple regression: testing and interpreting interactions. Sage, Newbury Park, CA

Google Scholar  

Alsulami H (2018) The effect of education and experience on wages: the case study of Saudi Arabia. Am J Ind Bus Manag 8(1):129–142

Ames C (1992) Classrooms: goals, structures, and student motivation. J Educ Psychol 84(3):261

Article   Google Scholar  

Amzaleg M, Masry-Herzallah A (2022) Cultural dimensions and skills in the 21st century: the Israeli education system as a case study. Pedagog Cult Soc 30(5):765–785

Arar K, Mustafa M (2011) Access to higher education for Palestinians in Israel. Educ Bus Soc 4(3):207–228

Atkinson JW (1957) Motivational determinants of risk-taking behavior. Psychol Rev 64 (6, Part 1), 359–372

Ayalon H, Blass N, Feniger Y, Shavit Y (2019) Educational inequality in Israel. From research to policy. Taub Center for Social Policy Studies in Israel, Jerusalem, Israel ‏

Baskakova DY, Belash OY, Shestopalov MY (2017) Graduates’ employment: expectations and reality. In: 2017 IEEE VI forum strategic partnership of universities and enterprises of hi-tech branches (Science. Education. Innovations) (SPUE). IEEE, St. Petersburg, Russia, pp. 128–131

Bautista MA, González F, Martinez LR, Munoz P, Prem M (2023) Dictatorship, higher education and social mobility. Working paper 2023-121. Becker Friedman Institute for Economics, University of Chicago‏

Blass N (2020) Achievements and gaps: the status of the Israeli education system. Taub Center for Social Policy Studies in Israel

Bowen WG, Bok D (1998) The shape of the river: long-term consequences of considering race in college and university admissions. Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ

Book   Google Scholar  

Bowen WG, Chingos MM, McPherson MS (2009) Crossing the finish line: Completing college at America’s public universities. Princeton University Press

Central Bureau of Statistics in Israel (2020) Demographic characteristics. Society in Israel, No. 12. https://www.cbs.gov.il/he/Statistical/statistical%20184mun.pdf . Accessed 22 Mar 2023

Charles CZ, Fischer MJ, Mooney MA, Massey DS (2009) Taming the river: negotiating the academic, financial, and social currents in selective colleges and universities. Princeton University Press

Dominguez-Whitehead Y (2017) Impediments to pursuing graduate studies in South Africa: the role of the family, social class, and race. J Comp Fam Stud 48(2):197–215

Dweck CS, Leggett EL (1988) A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality. Psychol Rev 95(2):256

Earley PC (1994) Self or group? Cultural effects of training on self-efficacy and performance. Adm Sci Q 39(1):89–117

Eliassy L (1999) The influence of the learning environment on the students’ goal orientation in the heterogeneous classroom [Unpublished master’s thesis]. Bar Ilan University Ramat-Gan, Israel

Elliot AJ, Trash TM (2001) Achievement goals and the hierarchical model of achievement motivation. Educ Psychol Rev 13:139–156

Feniger Y, Ayalon H, Mcdossi O (2013) Access to higher education among young people from the periphery: social life in Israel. Research report. Gandyr Foundation

Feniger Y, Bar-Haim E, Blank C (2021) From social origin to selective high school courses: ability grouping as a mechanism of securing social advantage in Israeli secondary education. Res Soc Stratif Mobil 76:100627

Guterman O, Neuman A (2019) Similar goals, different results: differences in group learning goals and their impact on academic achievements. J Furth High Educ 43(6):729–741. https://doi.org/10.1080/0309877X.2017.1394990

Guterman O, Gill E (2023) Differences between students of Jewish and Arab origin in reasons for deciding to study. Educ Urban Soc 55(1):119–137

Guterman O, Neuman A, Gill E (2024a) Learning goals and level of individualism among undergraduate students. Educ Inq. https://doi.org/10.1080/20004508.2024.2359750

Guterman O, Neuman A, Gill E (2024b) Support or interference? Support and academic achievements among majority and minority groups of undergraduate students studying together. Int J Changes Educ. https://doi.org/10.47852/bonviewIJCE42022264

Huang C (2012) Discriminant and criterion-related validity of achievement goals in predicting academic achievement: a meta-analysis. J Educ Psychol 104(1):48

Hung M, Smith WA, Voss MW, Franklin JD, Gu Y, Bounsanga J (2020) Exploring student achievement gaps in school districts across the United States. Educ Urban Soc 52(2):175–193

International Association for the Evaluation of Educational Achievement (2023) TIMSS 2023: mathematics and science achievement in Israel. https://www.iea.nl/publications/study-reports/timss-2023-mathematics-and-science-achievement-israel

Khattab N (2005) The effects of high school context and interpersonal factors on students’ educational expectations: a multi-level model. Soc Psychol Educ 8(1):19–40

Kugelmass H, Ready DD (2011) Racial/ethnic disparities in collegiate cognitive gains: a multilevel analysis of institutional influences on learning and its equitable distribution. Res High Educ 52(4):323–348

Lapidot-Lefler N, Hosri H (2016) Cyberbullying in a diverse society: comparing Jewish and Arab adolescents in Israel through the lenses of individualistic versus collectivist cultures. Soc Psychol Educ 19(3):569–585. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11218-016-9339-9

Lauri T, Põder K, Kunitsõn N (2022) Discrimination or explained differences? Individual and school-level effects explaining the minority achievement gap. J Baltic Stud 54(3):1–28

Lufi D, Parish-Plass JIM (2010) Differences between Arab and Jewish students in demographic and personality variables and their connection to academic success. Stud Educ 1/2:259–273

Maehr ML, Nicholls JG (1980) Culture and achievement motivation: a second look. In: Warren N (ed) Studies on cross-cultural psychology, vol. 2. Academic Press, New York, pp. 221–267

Martin ND, Spenner KI, Mustillo SA (2017) A test of leading explanations for the college racial-ethnic achievement gap: evidence from a longitudinal case study. Res High Educ 58(6):617–645

Meece JL, Anderman EM, Anderman LH (2006) Classroom goal structure, student motivation, and academic achievement. Annu Rev Psychol 57:487–503

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Mi-Ami N (2003) Background document for discussion: academic achievements in the Arab sector. Submitted to the Committee on the Rights of the Child. Knesset Research and Information Center. http://www.knesset.gov.il/committees/heb/material/data/2003-11-03.rtf . Accessed 7 Jul 2024

Mustafa M (2007) Changes in higher education among the Arab minority in Israel in the last decade. In: Arar H, Haj Yihye K (eds.) Academics and higher education among the Arabs in Israel: issues and dilemmas. Ramot, Tel Aviv, Israel, pp. 13–47

Nicholls JG (1992) Students as educational theorists. In: Schunk D, Meece J (eds.) Student perceptions in the classroom. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 267–286

OECD (2012) Untapped skills: realizing the potential of immigrant students. OECD Publishing, Paris

OECD (2018) Programme for International Student Assessment (PISA): results from PISA 2018. Country note: Israel. Equity related to socio-economic status. https://www.oecd.org/pisa/publications/PISA2018_CN_ISR.pdf

OECD (2019). The Road to Integration: Education and Migration. OECD Reviews of Migrant Education. OECD Publishing: Paris. https://doi.org/10.1787/d8ceec5d-en

Pérez-Martín M, Villardón-Gallego L (2023) University experiences of students in a gender minority. Sustainability 15(5):4054

Sagy S, Orr E, Bar-On D, Awwad E (2001) Individualism and collectivism in two conflicted societies: comparing Israeli–Jewish and Palestinian–Arab high school students. Youth Soc 33(1):3–30

U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics (2023) The condition of education 2023: achievement gaps. https://nces.ed.gov/pubsearch/pubsinfo.asp?pubid=2023222

Urdan T, Kaplan A (2020) The origins, evolution, and future directions of achievement goal theory. Contemp Educ Psychol 61:101862

Wigfield A, Eccles JS (2000) Expectancy-value theory of achievement motivation. Contemp Educ 25(1):68–81

CAS   Google Scholar  

Zuzovsky R (2008) Capturing the dynamics behind the narrowing achievement gap between Hebrew-speaking and Arabic-speaking schools in Israel: findings from TIMSS 1999 and 2003. Educ Res Eval 14(1):47–71

Download references

Author information

These authors contributed equally: Efrat Gill, Oz Guterman, Ari Neuman.

Authors and Affiliations

Department of Human Resources, Western Galilee College, Acre, Israel

Efrat Gill & Oz Guterman

Department of Education, Western Galilee College, Acre, Israel

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

All authors contributed equally to this article.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ari Neuman .

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval.

The research received approval from the Ethics Committee of Western Galilee College. #OG25718.

Informed consent

The researchers invited social sciences students to participate in the research during their classes at Western Galilee College. The researchers explained that participation was voluntary, and participants were assured that the data would not affect their grades or be used for any purpose other than the research. After the students gave their consent, in principle, to participate in the research, meetings were arranged to administer the questionnaires. The students signed an informed consent form to indicate their consent to participate in the research as well as permission for the research team to examine their final grades.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Gill, E., Guterman, O. & Neuman, A. Different motivation, different achievements: the relationship of motivation and dedication to academic pursuits with final grades among Jewish and Arab undergraduates studying together. Humanit Soc Sci Commun 11 , 1079 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-024-03548-7

Download citation

Received : 15 October 2023

Accepted : 01 August 2024

Published : 23 August 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1057/s41599-024-03548-7

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

what is motivation in a research paper

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it’s official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you’re on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings

Preview improvements coming to the PMC website in October 2024. Learn More or Try it out now .

  • Advanced Search
  • Journal List
  • J Adv Med Educ Prof
  • v.12(3); 2024 Jul
  • PMC11336194

Design and Effect of Neurosurgical Educational Software Using Gamification on Students' Learning and Motivation

Sedigheh hannani.

1 Department of Operating Room, School of Allied Medicine, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran

MAHDIEH SALEHI

Fardin amiri, nemam ali azadi.

2 Department of Biostatistics, School of Public Health, Iran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran

Introduction:

Gamification is an innovative learning approach that, when combined with technology, aligns well with students' preferences. Recognizing the necessity of employing effective educational methods in surgical team training, this study aims to examine the impact of gamification-based educational software on students' learning and motivation in the surgical technology field.

This study was conducted as a quasi-experimental single-group pre-and post-test design in Iran in 2023. The study sample comprised 40 undergraduate students majoring in surgical technology, selected through a census method. The educational software is designed according to the nine principles of the model of Karl Kapp and Sharon Buller, using the mechanics, dynamics and aesthetics (MDA) framework. After implementation, the software's impact on students' learning and motivation was assessed. The students' learning levels before and after using the educational software were evaluated using a multiple-choice test. To measure students' motivation, a researcher-developed questionnaire was utilized.  The Shapiro-Wilk test was used to check the normality of the distribution of the studied variables. Through SPSS version 26, descriptive statistics such as frequency, percentage, mean, and standard deviation were analyzed along with inferential statistics, including paired t-tests.

Among the participants, 55% were female, and the sample had a mean age of 22.30±0.99 years. A significant difference was observed between the mean learning scores before and after using the educational software (10.43±4.38 vs. 21±4.11, p < 0.001). The motivation level of the students showed a significant increase.

Conclusion:

Based on the results of this study, gamification-based software has led to improved learning and increased motivation among students in surgical technology.

Introduction

Neurosurgery, specifically brain and nerve surgery, is one of the most challenging and delicate surgical procedures ( 1 ). Medical errors within the surgical team can lead to irreversible complications such as mortality, iatrogenic diseases, and long-term post-neurosurgical complications ( 2 ). Surgical teams involved in neurosurgical procedures require fundamental surgical skills and a profound understanding of neuroanatomy and clinical manifestations of diseases ( 3 ). Despite the rapid advancements in surgical technology, surgical team training has been progressing much slower ( 4 ). One of the most significant challenges in educating surgical technology students is providing effective training that ensures optimal clinical services ( 5 ). In the surgical environment, where conditions change every second, surgical technologists must be trained efficiently to tackle the upcoming challenges effectively ( 6 ). With the emergence and progress of new technologies, the learning needs of digital-era learners have also transformed, rendering traditional teaching methods less effective ( 7 ).

Integrating technology into the educational process proves instrumental in meeting the requirements of the new generation, and one of the most recent technologies involves incorporating gamification techniques into electronic learning environments ( 8 ).

The concept of gamification was first introduced by Nick Pelling in 2002 and has recently gained widespread popularity ( 9 ). Studies indicate that this instructional method has increased tenfold in the past five years ( 10 ). Gartner, the company, defines gamification as using game elements in non-game environments ( 11 ). Implementing gamification in e-learning engages learners, enhances motivation, and improves student learning outcomes ( 12 ). This active and innovative learning approach aligns well with the learning preferences of medical students ( 13 ).

Gamification offers numerous advantages for healthcare service providers, including increased knowledge and job skills, the ability to repeat learning experiences, utilization in adult education, facilitation of the teaching-learning process, feedback provision, enhanced interaction between educators and learners, creating opportunities for participation and engagement, and boosting learners' interest and motivation ( 14 ). Increasing motivation also enhances the overall learning outcomes of learners ( 15 ). Motivation refers to an individual's inclination towards success and engagement in an activity, where success depends on an individual's ability and effort ( 16 ). According to studies, traditional teaching strategies cannot solve learners' lack of motivation ( 17 ). By incorporating various game elements, gamification significantly impacts learners' motivation ( 18 ). While many elements are introduced in effective gamification design, PBL (points, badges, and leaderboards) remains the most popular ( 19 ). One of the foundational theories explaining how gamification works in creating motivation is the self-determination theory presented by Ryan and Deci in 2000 ( 20 ). This theory focuses on three major psychological factors that lead to enthusiasm and motivation for tasks: competence, which refers to an individual's ability and power to adapt to conditions and adjust their effort and skills according to the difficulty level of the challenge; relatedness, which deals with changes in an individual's behavior concerning others and within social contexts; and autonomy, which emphasizes an individual's right to choose and decide without external pressures and constraints ( 21 ).

A review of related literature demonstrates the positive outcomes of gamification in various fields of the medical sciences ( 14 ). For instance, gamification in neurosurgery education improved motivation and performance among surgical residents. Learners in gamified environments exhibited higher speed and fewer errors than the control group ( 22 ). Another study conducted on health, laboratory sciences, and medicine, students showed higher motivation and learning levels in psychology education using gamification methods ( 23 ). Examination of students' feedback and electronic learning management system data indicated that gamification is an appropriate strategy for enhancing motivation among nursing students, although further studies are needed ( 24 ). Also, one study indicates improved learning among surgical technology students using gamification-based educational applications in heart surgery courses ( 25 ). Although adult education gamification studies are increasing, many questions remain unanswered ( 26 ). For instance, there is still uncertainty regarding gamification's effectiveness in various contexts and subject areas, and its effect on motivational outcomes has not remained consistent over time ( 27 , 28 ). Therefore, due to the insufficient evidence and the lack of attention given to the motivational components and player types of surgical technology students, the current study aims to design and investigate the effect of gamification-based neurosurgery software on the learning and motivation of surgical technology students at the Iran University of Medical Sciences (Tehran, Iran).

The present study was a quasi-experimental single-group pre-post-test design. 

Participants

The study was conducted on 40 undergraduate students in the operating room department at the Iran University of Medical Sciences. A gamified educational intervention was conducted in 2023 between May and July. The sampling method used was census sampling.

Inclusion and Exclusion

The inclusion criteria for this study were undergraduate students of the 6th and 8th semesters of surgical technology in the second semester of the academic year 2022-2023 who had successfully passed the theoretical courses on neurosurgery (offered in the 5th semester), expressed willingness to participate in the study, had not previously received training in gamification, were not guest or transfer students, and had access to a personal computer or smartphone. The exclusion criterion for the study was the failure to complete the software stages and questionnaires.

Design and Development of Educational Software

In this study, we applied the nine principles of effective educational game design proposed by Karl Kapp and Sharon Boller ( 29 ) to design and develop educational software for teaching neurosurgery techniques. This web-based software is versatile, compatible with various mobile phones and computers, and can be run on any operating system and screen size.

The stages are as follows:

Stage 1: Introduction and Familiarization with Gamification Fundamentals

In this stage, a deep understanding of the principles and concepts of the gaming and gamification domain was achieved through research, literature review, studying library resources, and conducting research studies.

Stage 2: Playing and Evaluating Educational Games

In this stage, educational games within the field of medical sciences (both domestic and international) were studied and evaluated. Technical and graphic design, game implementation platforms, educational content, etc., were all considered.

Stage 3: Exploration of Educational Games

During this stage, efforts were made to evaluate software and platforms based on gamification in medical sciences, especially focusing on their components and frameworks. The aim was to use the results and experiences from previous studies in the software design.

Stage 4: Defining the Learning Foundation and Initiating the Educational Design Process

Setting Objectives

In this stage, educational objectives were defined, and questions and questionnaires tailored to these objectives were designed and sent to the expert panel for evaluation.

Analysis of Learners' Characteristics

In this stage, demographic profile forms were completed by the participants. Additionally, the Hexad player types questionnaire was used to analyze the players' personalities and the specific components that should be considered alongside general components in game design ( 30 ). The predominant personality traits among the students were Philanthropists, free-spiritedness, and achievement. The components that received more attention alongside the general components included:

  • • Philanthropists: Assisting others and sharing knowledge; collecting medals; awareness of game objectives.
  • • Free-spirited: exploration, unlocking, the right to choose, being amazed.
  • • Achiever: challenges, certifications, learning opportunities, stages.
  • • General: game guidance and teaching to learners; feedback; theme; time constraints; rewards; storytelling; replay ability; leaderboard.

Fifth Stage: Linking Learning with Game Design

During this stage, the precise content of the game design document was developed using the Mechanics, Dynamics, and Aesthetics (MDA) framework. This is framework, introduced by Hunicke and colleagues in 2004, is one of the most fundamental and widely accepted frameworks for game design. Mechanics refers to the game's rules and principles (such as challenges, chances, and feedback). Dynamics encompasses the events during gameplay, including narrative, emotions, and progression paths. Aesthetics involve the emotions and feelings a player can experience while playing the game. These elements are essential for understanding systemic thinking and the mutual effects of game elements in non-game contexts ( 31 ).

Type of Game: electronic and web-based that can be run on computers and smartphones.

Goal: The goal of the game is to become a professional surgeon's assistant in the field of neurosurgery by getting the highest score and successfully passing all the stages.

Dynamic: The main dynamic is solving the problem and answering the questions.

Game Mechanics or Rules

Game environment: In this game, by creating a user account, learners must be able to complete each stage of the game, which is set based on the syllabus topics. The game consists of 9 stages; at the beginning, only the first stage will open, to begin with, and to open each stage, players need to successfully complete the previous stages by at least capturing one light. On the left side, the game narrator guides the learners to make decisions in the game environment.

Perform the steps: To pass each stage, at least one light equal to 30% of the points of that stage must be obtained. Otherwise, learners have to play the stage again from the beginning. The game was designed so that if the players needed to leave the game in the middle of each stage, they could answer the questions after returning to the game. Learners received feedback immediately after answering each question, and the correct answer was displayed.

Type of questions: To answer each question, the question was displayed first, and after clicking on "show options", the options containing correct and incorrect answers were displayed. After selecting the desired option or options, if the learner wants to correct his answer, he will click "again". If the answer is approved by clicking on "confirm," the answer will be validated, and the score will be calculated. There was a time limit to answer the questions.

Game Elements Used

  • • Point
  • • Story
  • • Time limitation
  • • levels
  • • leaderboard
  • • Badge
  • • Feedback
  • • Replay
  • • Competition
  • • Avatar

Educational content: The educational content related to neurosurgery techniques for the brain, spinal cord, and peripheral nerves was developed by the sources outlined in the undergraduate surgical technology curriculum ( 3 , 6 , 32 , 33 ). The stages of the curriculum encompass basic and advanced concepts, including anatomy, pathology, diagnostic procedures, surgical readiness, instruments and equipment, peripheral nerve surgery, brain surgery, and spinal surgery. The content of the course materials, instructional videos, and specialized terminology were also compiled at this stage. The faculty of the operating room department approved the validity of the educational content.

Sixth Stage: Considering Points and Rewards

In this software, various elements were utilized as rewards, including badges (earned by obtaining 95% of the points in each stage), a three-star system (based on the percentage of correct answers to questions), a score table (for comparing scores), cash prizes (awarded to the top 4 scorers), and certificates (for all individuals who successfully completed the game stages).

Seventh Stage: Prototype Development

In this stage, the game design document and the research team's ideas were shared with the programming team. Together, they collaborated to create the initial version of the software, aligning the game concept with the development process.

Eighth Stage: Playtesting and Iteration

Playtesting refers to the actions taken to improve the game until it reaches the desired state. After creating the initial prototype, the design team thoroughly reviewed the software. The entire process, from start to finish, was meticulously examined, and any necessary modifications were made by both the programmers and the design team. Furthermore, the software was made available to five target users to uncover and rectify any errors or issues in the design that might have gone unnoticed by the designers and programmers.

Ninth Stage: Game Development and Implementation, Iteration, and Deployment

In this stage, one of the researchers visited the research environment and conducted an orientation session on how to use the software and address potential questions from students. Subsequently, an information group was formed on a virtual network (Telegram), accessible to all students, to facilitate necessary communications. It's worth noting that an instructional video on how to use the software and supplementary information were provided within the group for students to refer to if needed. Students were then requested to message the researcher if they were interested in participating, and they would receive the link immediately. Students were given a two-month timeframe to complete the software. During the intervention, the researcher acted as a facilitator, providing necessary guidance and addressing any issues that arose while using the software. Since this software was entirely electronic and the instruction was provided virtually, students were self-directed in progressing through the stages, affording them the utmost flexibility in terms of time and location. After receiving the link and registering on the website, the students first participated in a pre-test to assess their initial knowledge. A week after completing the stages, they were given a post-test for evaluation. The samples were given the researcher's motivation questionnaire once they had finished using the software.

Data Collection Tools

Player Type: The hexad scale was used to assess the player type, introduced by Marczewski and colleagues in 2016 ( 30 ). This questionnaire consists of 24 items and employs a 5-point Likert scale (ranging from "strongly agree" with a score of 5 to "strongly disagree" with a score of 1). The hexad scale categorizes players in gamified environments into six personality types: philanthropists, socializers, free spirits, achievers, players, and disruptors. Each player type is assigned four specific questions, and the category with the highest score indicates the dominant player's personality. 

Assessment of Learning:  The researcher-designed test comprised 30 multiple-choice questions based on the reference sources of the undergraduate surgical technology curriculum. The questions covered topics related to the stages of surgery, including anatomy, clinical manifestations, diagnostic procedures, surgical preparedness, tools and equipment, peripheral nerve surgery, brain surgery, and spinal surgery. Each correct answer received one point, while incorrect or unanswered questions were scored zero. The maximum attainable score was 30, and the minimum was 0. Pre-test questions were completed before the educational intervention, and post-test questions were completed one week after the intervention.

Assessment of Motivation: A researcher-designed motivation questionnaire consisting of 10 items was provided to the students. The motivation questionnaire used a 5-point Likert scale (ranging from 1 for "completely disagree" to 5 for "completely agree"). Scores between 10 and 30 indicated low motivation, 31 to 40 indicated moderate motivation, and 41 to 50 indicated high motivation. The researcher's motivation questionnaire was provided to the samples after they completed the software's steps to measure the software's impact on students' motivation.

Validity and Reliability

Player-type questionnaire: The reliability and validity of this questionnaire in Iran were confirmed by Abdollahzade and Jafari ( 34 ), who obtained a Cronbach's alpha coefficient of 0.72. This study assessed internal consistency and reliability using Cronbach's alpha coefficient. The reliability coefficients were determined to be 0.856 for this questionnaire.

Learning question:   To ensure content validity, the content coverage and relevance of the question to the primary objectives were drafted according to the initial blueprint and Content Validity Ratio (CVR) and Content Validity Index (CVI) indices approved by 10 experts in the fields of surgical technology, medical education, and e-learning. We asked an expert to rate each of the items as ‘‘essential’’, ‘‘useful but not essential’’, or ‘‘not essential’’. A weighted value was assigned to each rating. Responses from all panelists were pooled, and the number indicating ‘‘essential’’ for each item was determined. The value of the content validity ratio for 10 experts in the Lawshe table is considered to be at least 0.62. Items that did not reach this threshold were deleted from the final questionnaire. This ratio was in the range of 0.8 to 1 for the final questionnaire items for the CVR.  For the Content Validity Index (CVI), the three criteria of relevance, simplicity, and clarity were examined separately by experts in a four-part spectrum for each of the items (1: unrelated, 2: somewhat relevant, 3: relevant, and 4: completely relevant). The CVI score was calculated by summing the positive scores for each item ranked 3rd and 4th over the total number of experts. Acceptance of items based on CVI was higher than 0.79 ( 35 , 36 ). Construct validity was confirmed by verifying the formulation of questions and options for each question based on the Millman checklist. A Cronbach's alpha coefficient of 0.870 was obtained for this questionnaire.

Motivation Questionnaire: The content validity (CVR and CVI indices) of this questionnaire was confirmed by 10 experts in surgical technology, medical education, and e-learning. The value of the content validity ratio was in the range of 0.8 to 1 for the questionnaire items. The content validity index of all questionnaire items was above 0.79. Cronbach's alpha coefficient was determined to be 0.948 for the motivation questionnaire.

Statistical Analysis: The results of the Shapiro-Wilk test indicated that the distribution of the study variables is normal ( Table 1 ). Parametric tests were employed for data analysis. Data were analyzed using descriptive statistics such as frequency, percentage, mean, and standard deviation, as well as inferential statistics, including paired t-tests, in SPSS version 26. A significance level of less than 0.05 was considered. 

Shapiro-Wilk test to determine the normality of Variables

Variable’sMean±SDShapiro Wilk statisticsP
Learning (before)10.43±4.380.960 0.168
Learning (after)21±4.110.950 0.079
Motivation39.53±8.340.9310.091

Based on the data collected from 40 research participants, 55% (22 individuals) were female, and 45% (18 individuals) were male. The average age of the sample was 22.30±0.99. As observed in Table 2 , the level of gaming personality traits among surgical technology students has been significantly high in the areas of altruism, freedom, and achievement.

Types of Player Personality Among Surgical Technology Students

DomainScoreN (%)Mean±SD
PhilanthropistsLow (scores between 4 to 12)5 (12.8%)11 ± 1.73
Average (scores between 13 to 16)13 (33.3%)15.62 ± 0.87
High (scores between 17 to 20)21 (53.8%)19.10 ± 1.18
Total39 (100%)16.90 ± 3.02
SocialLow (scores between 4 to 12)8 (20.5%)9.38 ± 1.30
Average (scores between 13 to 16)17 (43.6%)14.76 ± 1.09
High (scores between 17 to 20)14 (35.9%)18.79 ± 1.42
Total39 (100%)15.10 ± 3.67
Free-spiritedLow (scores between 4 to 12)2 (5.1%)11 ± 1.4
Average (scores between 13 to 16)12 (30.8%)15 ± 1.35
High (scores between 17 to 20)25 (64.1%)18.40 ± 1.12
Total39 (100%)16.97 ± 2.41
DisruptersLow (scores between 4 to 12)19 (48.7%)9.63 ± 2.31
Average (scores between 13 to 16)16 (41%)14.44 ± 1.15
High (scores between 17 to 20)4 (10.3%)18.50 ± 1.29
Total39 (100%)12.51 ± 3.56
AchieverLow (scores between 4 to 12)1 (2.6%)11 ± 0
Average (scores between 13 to 16)12 (30.8%)14.75 ± 1.05
High (scores between 17 to 20)26 (66.7%)18.62 ± 1.20
Total39 (100%)17.23 ± 2.36
PlayerLow (scores between 4 to 12)2 (5.1%)12 ± 0
Average (scores between 13 to 16)21 (53.8%)14.48 ± 1.08
High (scores between 17 to 20)16 (41%)18.75 ± 1.29
Total39 (100%)16.10 ± 2.56

As observed in Table 3 , there was a statistically significant difference in the mean scores obtained from the questions related to learning among surgical technology students before and after training (p < 0.001), with the mean scores approximately doubling.

Comparison of the Mean Scores Obtained from Questions Related to Learning Among Surgical Technology Students Before and After Training

TimeNMean±SDTest statistic P
Before training4010.43±4.38t=-11.51<0.001
After training4021±4.11

As indicated in Table 4 , after the educational intervention, the level of motivation among surgical technology students was predominantly high (19 individuals, equivalent to 47%) and moderate (15 individuals, equivalent to 37.5%).

Motivation Level of Surgical Technology Students

ScoreN (%)Mean±SD
Low (scores between 10 to 30)6 (15%)25.17 ± 5.64
Average (scores between 31 to 40)15 (37.5%)36.47 ± 3.04
High (scores between 41 to 50)19 (47.5%)46.47 ± 2.72
Total40 (100%)39.53 ± 8.34

The mean and percentage of students' responses to the motivation questionnaire are shown in Table 5 .

Mean and Percentage of Students' Responses to the Motivation Questionnaire

Motivation ItemStrongly disagree (1)Disagree (2)No opinion (3)Agree (4)Strongly agree (5)SD±Mean
n (%)n (%)n (%)n (%)
1.By earning a higher score, i gain a higher level of scientific competence.1 (2.5%)08 (20%)17 (42.5%)14 (35%)4.08± 0.888
2.Completing various stages leads to my satisfaction.0(5%) 2(17.5%) 715 (37.5%)(40%) 164.12±0.883
3.Receiving immediate feedback boosts my motivation to rectify mistakes.0(5%) 2(17.5%) 7(45%) 1832%)) 134.05±0.846
4.I strive to improve my performance by comparing my rank in the score table (leaderboard).(2.5%) 1(.5%7) 3(12.5%) 5(35%) 14(42.5%) 174.08 ± 1.047
5.Completing challenges gives me greater self-confidence in the operating room environment.(5%) 2(5%) 232.5%)) 1330%)) 12(27.5%) 113.70±1.091
6.By earning a higher score, I acquire more practical competence.(2.5%) 1(5%) 2(22.5%) 9(45%)18 (25%) 103.85±0.949
7.Using the software motivates me to put in more effort for personal and professional growth.(5%) 2(2.5%) 1(30%) 12(27.5%) 11(35%) 143.85±1.099
8.Completing the stages increases my interest in participating in surgeries.(7.5%)3 (2.5%) 1(25%) 10(32.5%) 13(32.5%) 133.80±1.159
9.I put all my effort into successfully completing my learning process.(5%)2 (2.5%)1 (22.5%) 9(32.5%) 13(37.5%)15 3.95±1.085
10.Implementing this teaching method enhances my self-directed learning.(2.5%)1 (5%) 2(20%) 8(30%) 12(42.5%) 174.05±1.037

The results of this study demonstrate that game-based educational software has impacted the learning outcomes of surgical technology students, leading to increased learning. These findings align with the research conducted by Salehinia and colleagues (2023), where they designed, implemented, and evaluated a game-based educational application for cardiac surgery on surgical technology students. The results of this study have shown that with the advancement of technology, the use of innovative methods such as gamification in education leads to improved learning outcomes and enhances the effectiveness of education for surgical technology students ( 25 ). Furthermore, the results of this study align with the research conducted by Mosalanejad and colleagues (2018), which aimed to investigate the effectiveness of game-based education on the learning indices of nursing students. The findings of their study demonstrated that gamification, by creating an interactive and engaging environment, significantly impacts students' learning outcomes ( 37 ). Additionally, our study findings align with the research conducted by McAuliffe and colleagues (2020). Their study aimed to assess the feasibility and effectiveness of game-based education for general surgery residents, revealing that using this instructional method led to enhanced learning outcomes (increasing the average scores from 28 to 43) ( 38 ).

Furthermore, the study by Eslami and colleagues (2020), which aimed to design a game-based educational application for basic drug information targeted at pharmacy students, demonstrated that this application served as a useful tool for learning drug-related information. This instructional method can be employed across various academic disciplines ( 39 ). The traditional roles of teachers and students in teaching and learning have changed nowadays, emphasizing the active participation of learners. When students actively engage in their learning process and construct meaning proactively, a better and deeper learning experience is achieved ( 40 ). Therefore, gamification is among the most active and student-centered educational approaches. It enhances the quality of learning by creating a dynamic environment with visual appeal.

The results of this study also indicate that most surgical technology students (85%) had a high motivation for learning through gamification. Numerous studies have demonstrated that incorporating game elements enhances student motivation. The findings of Mosalanejad and colleagues ( 37 ), who conducted a study to evaluate gamification in the field of psychiatry on a group of medical and paramedical students, showed that when educational content is combined with appropriate game elements, it significantly boosts learners' motivation (mean>2.5) ( 21 ). Furthermore, Permanasari and colleagues (2021) conducted a study in Indonesia to teach anatomy to medical science students through gamification. They concluded that educational media based on gamification, incorporating elements such as rankings and scores, significantly enhances students' motivation ( 41 ). The study conducted by Felszeghy and colleagues (2019) aimed to examine the impact of the gamified online platform (Kahoot) on medical and dental students in the histology course. The study demonstrated that gamification increased motivation for learning in 77.5% of the participants ( 42 ). Based on the findings of our study and in line with our self-determination theory, we found that learners, by answering questions and facing challenges presented in the educational software and progressing through stages to acquire the necessary knowledge, felt a sense of competence and capability in the scientific (77.5%) and practical (70%) aspects. They expressed that the software effectively increased their participation in the operating room environment (65%). Regarding learner interaction, due to the elements present in gamification, such as immediate feedback (77%) and a leaderboard (77.5%), participants made efforts to improve their performance and rectify errors. Furthermore, learners expressed that they put in their utmost effort to succeed in the learning process within the software (70%). Regarding autonomy, given the flexibility of time and place in the software and its student-centered nature, learners could guide their learning process without external pressure or constraints (72.5%). Despite the positive effects of gamification in health profession education, some studies have also pointed out unintended consequences ( 10 ). For example, some studies have mentioned that certain components, like competition and scoring tables, might decrease motivation for some students ( 43 , 44 ). The results obtained in these studies could be because students with weaker performance may feel discouraged and demotivated when they see their low rank and score compared to others. Another reason for decreased motivation could be the lack of attention to motivational components tailored to learners' preferences and personality traits. The findings of the present study indicated that elements such as points (77.5%), immediate feedback (77%), leaderboards (77.5%), stages (77.5%), and challenges (57.5%) were effective in increasing student motivation. Therefore, considering that most studies demonstrate the positive impact of gamification on learners' motivation levels, it can be concluded that when educational content is combined with appropriate game elements, and learners have control over their learning process, they will exhibit high motivation.

 According to the results of the present study, the students' predominant personality traits were Philanthropists, free-spiritedness, and achievement. We tried to use game components suitable for the type of player in the gamification design. In various studies, students' game personalities have been investigated. For example, in a study by Kocadere and Çağlar (2018), player types were determined as killer, achiever, explorer, and socializer ( 45 ). In the study by Krath and von Korflesch (2021), athletes and philanthropists were the most dominant ( 46 ). Certainly, according to demographic characteristics (such as age, gender, field of study, etc.) and educational content, students have individual player types. It is important to pay attention to the personality characteristics of students in gamification design to create motivation and better learning.

Strengths and Limitations

In this study, we endeavored to design game-based educational software in an effective and student-centered manner, considering the personality traits of widespread gamers and the nine gamification principles. The web-based educational software we developed was compatible with various operating systems. We also extracted several motivational components and their impact percentages on students. It was a prerequisite for the participants to have completed a comprehensive theoretical unit on brain and nerve surgery before engaging with the software. The research sample was limited to 6th and 8th-semester surgical technology students at Iran University of Medical Sciences. Also, one of the limitations of our study was that it was carried out in a single group.  

Recommendations for Future Studies

Future studies can be conducted with a larger sample size and the inclusion of a control group. Additionally, it is possible to utilize game-based software in blended learning and face-to-face instruction.

The results of this study demonstrated that game-based software significantly influenced the enhancement of learning and increased motivation among surgical technology students. In today's world, technology has impacted all fields, and education is no exception. Technology creates new opportunities for educating digital-native students. Gamification is an innovative and active educational approach that optimizes and makes learning engaging, offering substantial potential for fostering self-directed learning in learners. Integrating technology and gamification elements in e-learning environments enhances the enjoyment of learning and improves learners' motivation, potentially positively affecting personal and professional development. Educational specialists and designers can apply the findings of this research in all medical-related fields, especially surgical technology. Further studies in this area are warranted to explore its full potential.

Ethical Considerations and Acknowledgments

This research was conducted under the ethical code IR.IUMS.RES.1402.102, approved by the Operating Room Department of the Iran University of Medical Sciences, as part of a Master's thesis. The financial support provided by the Research Deputy of this university is gratefully acknowledged. The researchers now express their sincere gratitude to the students who participated in this study.

Authors’ Contribution

Conceptualization and supervision: S.H. and M.S. Data collection: M.S.; statistical analysis: N.A.A.; investigation: M.S.; methodology: F.A. and M.S.; resources: M.S.; software: M.S. and S.H. The final manuscript has been read and approved by all authors, who agree to be accountable for all aspects of the work.

Conflict of Interest

The authors reported no potential conflicts of interest.

  • Open access
  • Published: 22 August 2024

Factors influencing fidelity to guideline implementation strategies for improving pain care at cancer centres: a qualitative sub-study of the Stop Cancer PAIN Trial

  • Tim Luckett 1 ,
  • Jane Phillips 2 ,
  • Meera Agar 1 , 3 ,
  • Linda Richards 4 ,
  • Najwa Reynolds 5 ,
  • Maja Garcia 1 ,
  • Patricia Davidson 6 ,
  • Tim Shaw 7 ,
  • David Currow 6 ,
  • Frances Boyle 8 , 9 ,
  • Lawrence Lam 10 ,
  • Nikki McCaffrey 11 &
  • Melanie Lovell 5 , 9  

BMC Health Services Research volume  24 , Article number:  969 ( 2024 ) Cite this article

27 Accesses

Metrics details

The Stop Cancer PAIN Trial was a phase III pragmatic stepped wedge cluster randomised controlled trial which compared effectiveness of screening and guidelines with or without implementation strategies for improving pain in adults with cancer attending six Australian outpatient comprehensive cancer centres ( n  = 688). A system for pain screening was introduced before observation of a ‘control’ phase. Implementation strategies introduced in the ‘intervention’ phase included: (1) audit of adherence to guideline recommendations, with feedback to clinical teams; (2) health professional education via an email-administered ‘spaced education’ module; and (3) a patient education booklet and self-management resource. Selection of strategies was informed by the Capability, Opportunity and Motivation Behaviour (COM-B) Model (Michie et al., 2011) and evidence for each strategy’s stand-alone effectiveness. A consultant physician at each centre supported the intervention as a ‘clinical champion’. However, fidelity to the intervention was limited, and the Trial did not demonstrate effectiveness. This paper reports a sub-study of the Trial which aimed to identify factors inhibiting or enabling fidelity to inform future guideline implementation initiatives.

The qualitative sub-study enabled in-depth exploration of factors from the perspectives of personnel at each centre. Clinical champions, clinicians and clinic receptionists were invited to participate in semi-structured interviews. Analysis used a framework method and a largely deductive approach based on the COM-B Model.

Twenty-four people participated, including 15 physicians, 8 nurses and 1 clinic receptionist. Coding against the COM-B Model identified ‘capability’ to be the most influential component, with ‘opportunity’ and ‘motivation’ playing largely subsidiary roles. Findings suggest that fidelity could have been improved by: considering the readiness for change of each clinical setting; better articulating the intervention’s value proposition; defining clinician roles and responsibilities, addressing perceptions that pain care falls beyond oncology clinicians’ scopes of practice; integrating the intervention within existing systems and processes; promoting patient-clinician partnerships; investing in clinical champions among senior nursing and junior medical personnel, supported by medical leaders; and planning for slow incremental change rather than rapid uptake.

Conclusions

Future guideline implementation interventions may require a ‘meta-implementation’ approach based on complex systems theory to successfully integrate multiple strategies.

Trial registration

Registry: Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry; number: ACTRN 12615000064505; data: https://www.anzctr.org.au/Trial/Registration/TrialReview.aspxid=367236&isReview=true .

Peer Review reports

Pain is a common and burdensome symptom in people with cancer [ 1 ]. Barriers to pain care occur at all ‘levels’, including the patient and family (e.g., misconceptions regarding opioids), clinician (e.g. lack of expertise), service (e.g. inadequate referral processes) and healthcare system (e.g. lack of coordination) [ 2 , 3 , 4 , 5 , 6 , 7 , 8 ]. A recent systematic review suggests that around 40% of cancer patients with pain may not receive adequate management [ 9 ]. Research has demonstrated that routine screening and implementation of evidence-based guidelines has potential to improve quality of cancer pain care and outcomes [ 10 , 11 , 12 , 13 , 14 ]. However, experience suggests that clinicians are unlikely to utilise screening results or follow guidelines unless these are supported by targeted strategies [ 15 , 16 ].

The Stop Cancer PAIN Trial (ACTRN 12615000064505) was a phase III pragmatic stepped wedge cluster randomised controlled trial conducted between 2014 and 2019 which compared the effectiveness of screening and guidelines with or without implementation strategies for improving pain in adults with cancer attending six outpatient comprehensive cancer centres in Australia ( n  = 688) [ 17 , 18 ]. A pen/paper system to screen for pain using 0–10 numerical rating scales (NRS) for worst and average intensity over the past 24 h was introduced to each centre prior to observation of a ‘control’ phase, in which clinicians were also made aware of the Australian Cancer Pain Management in Adults guidelines [ 19 ]. At the beginning of the training phase, trial investigators presented at staff meetings on the importance of better managing pain and the rationale and evidence base for the intervention components. Implementation strategies (collectively termed the ‘intervention’) were then introduced in a ‘training’ phase and maintained during an ‘intervention’ phase as follows: (1) audit of adherence to key guideline recommendations [ 19 ] and feedback delivered to clinical teams in one or two cycles; (2) health professional education via a ‘Qstream’ email-administered ‘spaced education’ module [ 20 ]; and (3) a patient education booklet and self-management resource for completion together with a clinician that included goal setting, a pain diary and pain management plan [ 21 , 22 ]. Selection of these strategies was informed by the Capability, Opportunity and Motivation Behaviour (COM-B) Model of behaviour change [ 23 ], and evidence that each strategy had been separately effective for supporting guideline implementation for other health conditions. The intervention was supported at each centre by a consultant physician who agreed to be a ‘clinical champion’ [ 24 ].

As reported previously [ 18 ], the Stop Cancer PAIN Trial found no significant differences between the intervention and the control phases on the trial’s primary outcome - the proportion of patients with moderate-severe worst pain intensity who reported a 30% decrease at 1-week follow-up. Fidelity to the intervention was lower than anticipated and variable between centres: only 2/6 centres had two audit cycles rather than one; completion rates for the health professional spaced education varied from 12% to 74% between centres; and the proportion of patients reporting receipt of written information of any kind rose to an average of only 30% (20-44%) versus 22% (2-30%) in the control phase. Unexpectedly, secondary measures of mean, worst and average pain over a 4-week follow-up period improved by 0.5 standard deviation during control as well as intervention phases. However, the lack of a comparison group with no screening system made it difficult to conclude whether improvement in the control phase was due to effects from screening, a Hawthorne effect, or some other explanation.

The current paper reports a sub-study of the Stop Cancer PAIN Trial which aimed to identify factors influencing fidelity to the intervention that might warrant consideration by similar initiatives in the future.

The intervention, methods and results of the Stop Cancer PAIN trial have been described in previous open-access articles [ 17 , 18 ]. The sub-study used a qualitative approach with pragmatic orientation to enable in-depth exploration of factors influencing success from the perspectives of clinicians at each participating centre [ 25 ]. Clinician views canvassed at interview were considered the most efficient means of identifying barriers and enablers among complex contextual factors at each centre, including personnel’s knowledge, attitudes and beliefs towards pain care and the intervention.

The sub-study was approved by the Southwestern Sydney Local Health District Human Research Ethics Committee (HREC/14/LPOOL/479) as part of the overall trial. All participants gave written informed consent to participate.

Reporting adheres to the consolidated criteria for reporting qualitative research (COREQ) [ 26 ].

Participants

Participants were eligible if they were employed on a permanent basis either full- or part-time at a participating centre in a role that provided clinical care to cancer patients or patient-focused administrative support. The clinical champion at each centre was invited to participate by the research team. Other personnel were invited by means of email circulars and verbal invitations during meetings. Given the diverse range of roles at each centre, no limit was set on sample size to canvass as many perspectives as possible.

Data collection

Data were collected by means of semi-structured interviews conducted by one of two researchers, a female pharmacist with experience of medical education for pain management (LR), and a male social scientist with a doctorate (TL). Both interviewers had prior experience in qualitative research and knew some participants through their project roles.

Participants were fully aware of the study purpose before consenting. Interviews were conducted face-to-face or by telephone, with the participant and interviewer being the only people present. Interviews began with open questions about ‘what worked’ and ‘didn’t work’ across the intervention before focusing on each implementation strategy in more detail and important contextual factors at their centre (see Table  1 for a topic guide, which was developed specifically for this study). Interviewers explicitly invited criticism, expressing a tone of open enquiry and neutrality throughout. Prompts were used as necessary to explore factors identified by participants in more detail. Factors identified at previous interviews were raised at subsequent ones for verification, inviting participants to disagree or agree as they felt appropriate. No requests were received to return transcripts to participants for comment. Interviews were audio-recorded and transcribed verbatim.

Analysis used the framework method [ 27 ] and a largely deductive approach based on the same theoretical framework used during intervention design - the COM-B Model [ 23 ]. Based on a systematic review, the COM-B Model posits that behaviour change requires three conditions, namely ‘capability’ (including both psychological and physical capacity), ‘opportunity’ (all the factors that lie outside the individual that make the behaviour possible or prompt it) and ‘motivation’ (including habitual processes, emotional responding, as well as analytical decision-making). Initial line-by-line coding categorized data against these conditions according to which best described relationships between factors and behaviours within and across implementation strategies and the levels of patient, clinician and centre. While the COM-B model originally focused directly on human behaviour, it became clear during coding that behaviour was substantially influenced by centre, specialty and disciplinary factors, so these were also considered appropriate foci for coding against COM conditions. To enhance credibility, the same data were coded in different ways where multiple interpretations seemed plausible until coding of further interviews identified consistencies to help with disambiguation. Charting of codes for data within and between centres enabled mapping between the relative contributions made by each condition, summarised as lessons learned for guiding similar initiatives in the future. Dependability was increased by ensuring coding was conducted by two members of the research team (NR, MG) who had no previous involvement in the project but were experienced in qualitative research. Review and discussion with two team members who were involved in the project throughout (TL and ML) was intended to balance ‘outsider’ and ‘insider’ perspectives to guard against bias from preconceived interpretations whilst also referencing contextual understanding. Both Excel 2019 (Microsoft) and NVivo V12 (QSR) software were used to help manage different stages of the analytic process.

Twenty-four people participated across the six centres, ranging from one to six participants. Fifteen were physicians (of whom six were clinical champions), eight were nurses, and one was a clinic receptionist. This response rate ranged from 2 to 27% of eligible personnel at each centre. See Table  2 for a more detailed summary of participant roles at each centre. Interviews were a median of 20 min long, with an inter-quartile range of 13 to 28 min.

Capability, opportunity and motivation

Coding against the COM-B Model identified ‘capability’ to be the component having most influence over intervention success, with ‘opportunity’ and ‘motivation’ playing largely subsidiary roles.

Capabilities: Pertinent capabilities were reported to include: a pre-existing, centre-level culture of continuous improvement, communication pathways between senior management and other personnel, established roles and responsibilities for pain care among disciplines and specialties, systems and processes that could readily accommodate the intervention, and a culture of involving patients as partners in care. These capabilities influenced the degree to which personnel and patients had the opportunity and motivation to fully engage with the intervention.

Opportunity and motivation: These elements were most frequently discussed by participants in terms of ‘time’ that personnel could commit to pain care relative to other responsibilities. Clinical champions were perceived to play a critical role in supporting intervention success but were under-resourced at every centre and challenged by turnover in the role at two. In addition to more systemic drivers, individual personnel’s motivation was influenced by the degree to which they accepted the intervention’s value proposition at the outset and perceived this to be demonstrated over time.

Interactions between capability, opportunity and motivation are explored below in terms of their implications for similar future initiatives. Findings suggest that fidelity could have been improved by: considering the readiness for change of each clinical setting; better articulating the intervention’s value proposition; defining clinician roles and responsibilities, addressing perceptions that pain care falls beyond oncology clinicians’ scopes of practice; integrating the intervention within existing systems and processes; promoting patient-clinician partnerships; investing in clinical champions among senior nursing and junior medical personnel, supported by medical leaders; and planning for slow incremental change rather than rapid uptake.

Consider centres’ readiness for change

The degree to which centres had a pre-existing culture of continuous improvement was considered important in providing a fertile context for the intervention. At Centre 5, there was a consensus that change of any kind was difficult to instigate, even according to the head of department: “… because it’s new - because we’re so entrenched in our ways ” (C5P04 [Centre 5, participant 04] medical oncologist, head of department and clinical champion). At another, the complex centre-level nature of the intervention was perceived to pose particular challenges compared to oncology drug trials with which they were more familiar: “ we haven’t been a principal site [in a trial of this kind] previously and I think that’s sort of opened up some gaps in knowledge for us and some opportunities for learning in the future … what kind of support we’d need to come with that trial to help it be a success in this culture ” (C3P02 palliative care physician and clinical champion).

Articulate and deliver on the intervention’s value proposition

Interviews highlighted the importance of articulating the intervention’s value proposition to every member of the workforce and maintaining engagement by demonstrating benefits over time. At Centre 5, some participants perceived that the intervention had been imposed by management rather than generated from clinical priorities: “…senior staff say [to researchers] ‘come to our clinics, but we expect everyone else to do the work’ ” (C5P05 radiation oncologist). This was compounded by a perceived lack of communication about the project, which limited personnels’ opportunity to take a more active role even when they were motivated to do so: “ I would have facilitated [the intervention] … but I didn’t know about it ” (C5P01 nurse practitioner). Eliciting and maintaining engagement was said to be additionally challenged at this centre by high staff turnover, especially among junior medical officers on rotation: “ it was very accepted by the junior medical staff [but] I think, unfortunately, when there’s a relatively high turnover of staff … ” (C5P07 radiation oncology trainee). At two other centres, turnover among personnel required a transition in the role of clinical champion, interrupting support for the intervention while the new incumbents familiarised themselves with the role.

Across centres, participants reported reservations among some of their colleagues regarding the project’s fundamental premises, including the assumption that pain care needed improving at their centre (“ they actually felt this trial was a little bit insulting for their clinical skills. There was a bit of eye rolling and ‘of course we do that already!’ ” (C3P02 palliative care physician and clinical champion)) or that pain warranted a specific focus rather than symptoms more generally: “ I find it more useful when more than one symptom is targeted ” (C5P06 palliative care physician).

More specific criticism was also levelled at each of the intervention strategies as follows.

Pain screening

In the case of screening, two participants questioned the validity of a 0–10 numerical rating scale (NRS) for different reasons: “ sometimes getting the numbers breaks the flow of the narrative” (C6P04 medical oncologist); “they [patients] would say, ‘no, I’m not in pain but I have a lot of discomfort when I swallow’ - it was in the wording ” C5P02 registered nurse). Even one of the clinical champions felt that screening was redundant where pain was very severe: “ if someone is clearly in a pain crisis, you don’t need to be asking … you kind of know what number - they might tell you it’s 15 [out of 10] ” (C6P02 palliative care physician and clinical champion). Perceptions of the value of screening were also influenced by the degree to which it led to demonstrable improvements in pain care, which was undermined by problems with establishing an efficient process at some centres: “ I think I’ve still probably got stray [pain screening] forms on my desk ” (C3P06 palliative care physician). A lack of understanding among personnel and patients about how screening might lead to better pain outcomes was said to result in “ fatigue ” (C5P03 clinical nurse consultant [clinical nurse consultant]; C1P01 palliative care physician and clinical champion), manifest as a downward spiral of effort in, and value from, screening.

Audit and feedback

The audit and feedback strategy attracted limited attention from personnel at most centres: “ I don’t think that the audit and feedback were terribly noticeable ” (C4P01 medical oncologist and clinical champion). At the centre where only the palliative care department participated, one participant perceived baseline audit results to be acceptable and therefore demotivating for change: “[ the audit results showed] we were doing a good job even ahead of time … it did sort of make you think – ‘well where do we go from here?’ ” (C6P04 pain medicine physician). At another centre, motivation among personnel to improve on less favourable audit findings was perceived to depend on whether they prioritised pain care to start with: “ people have come up to me and said, ‘Gee, we really did very badly didn’t we?’ … but they’re not necessarily the people who don’t treat pain well - that’s the problem ” (C1P01 palliative care physician and clinical champion).

Spaced education for health professionals

Participants’ opinion on the value of the online spaced education depended on discipline and seniority, with nurses and junior medical officers reporting benefits “( it gave me a bit more confidence that I was on the right track” (C5P01 nurse practitioner)) but consultant physicians perceiving the knowledge level too “basic” (C6P04 pain medicine physician) or questioning advice from online spaced education that their responses were ‘wrong’: “…some of the multiple answers could have been equally valid” (C504 medical oncologist and clinical champion). Where consultants remained engaged, motivation was said to rely on cultivating “ competition” between colleagues (C602 palliative care physician and clinical champion). Inevitably, the voluntary nature of online spaced education also meant that only motivated personnel engaged to begin with.

Patient self-management resource

All participants who had used the patient self-management resource perceived at least some value. However, its use was limited by barriers relating to role and process considered below.

Define roles and responsibilities

Among the most commonly voiced barriers was a lack of clarity about which specialties and disciplines should be responsible for pain screening, patient education and management. This was usually described in terms of a ‘lack of time’ for pain care relative to other duties afforded greater priority within their scope of practice. Perspectives on roles and responsibilities are considered separately for each aspect of pain care as follows.

While most centres allocated the clinical task of pain screening to clinic receptionists, there was widespread reflection that this had been suboptimal. The only participating clinic receptionist felt that pain screening fell outside her area of responsibility: “but I’m an administrative person - I don’t have anything to do with pain management ” (C2P03 clinic receptionist). Clinician participants across disciplines similarly perceived that pain screening required clinical expertise to assist patients with reporting their pain and triage for urgent follow-up: “ you need somebody talking to the patients, rather than just handing the form, say ‘fill this in’ ” (C2P04 clinical nurse consultant). One centre that recognised this early on reallocated screening from an administrative to a nursing role, leading to substantial improvements in the completeness and quality of data: “ it made a big difference and certainly improved our ability to recognise people who had pain and allowed access for those people who were in severe pain to medications or at least an assessment … implementation through the clerical staff was not a long-term strategy ” (C1P01 palliative care physician and clinical champion).

Patient education

There was little consensus on which disciplines should be responsible for supporting patients to use the self-management resource, with medical personnel deferring to nurses and vice-versa. Role allocation was challenged by the diverse components within the resource, with each perceived to fall within a different scope of practice: “ pain is something I always do as an assessment … [but] … I’m not managing the pain … I’ll review and make recommendations and talk about the pain diaries and discussing their diary with their palliative care doctor or their general practitioner. And I would encourage that process. [But] I wouldn’t be the one that’s setting the goals on their daily activities and stuff ” (C5P01 nurse practitioner). Some oncology nursing roles were perceived to focus on chemo- or radiotherapy protocols to the exclusion of supportive care unless symptoms arose from, or impeded, treatment. Meanwhile, oncologists tended to interpret their role as solely focused on prescribing rather than also encompassing patient education: “ junior doctors only [have] 15 minutes to take a history and everything. [They] could enter in meds [into the patient resource] if everything else is done by someone else … part of me knows it’s [patient resource] important, but the other part of me - I just - when will I have time in my clinical practice to do it? ” (C5P05 radiation oncologist).

Pain management

Some oncologists viewed even pharmacological pain management as peripheral to their scope of practice when consultation time was short, prioritising cancer treatment instead. These participants viewed their role as limited to referring to palliative medicine or pain specialists, especially where pain was believed to have causes other than cancer: “ if the pain is a complex pain where the patient doesn’t have evidence of cancer, and it may be treatment-related, then in those scenarios we tend to divert to the chronic pain team ” (C5P07 radiation oncology advanced trainee). While participants from palliative care and pain medicine welcomed referrals for complex cases, they felt that oncologists sometimes referred for pain they could have easily managed themselves: “ what about some regular paracetamol? … These are things that you’d expect any junior doctors, never mind consultants [to have provided advice on] ” (C5P06 palliative care physician).

Integrate within existing systems and processes

Participants from several centres expressed a view that the intervention’s complex nature had proven overwhelming for systems and processes at their centres. At two centres, integration was especially challenged by broader infrastructure shifts and process failures that limited receptiveness to further changes. Participants at several centres emphasised the process-driven nature of oncology services and the challenge of changing established processes: “ they have got a pro forma that they use for chemo-immunotherapy review, and pain is not part of it, and that perhaps needs more of an organisational nuance … why doesn’t pain feature as a clinical outcome as part of the chemotherapy, immunotherapy review?” (C6P01 clinical nurse consultant). Participants emphasised the need to integrate pain care into existing processes to help personnel understand what was expected of them: “…nursing staff were getting them [screening forms] in the patient’s files and going, ‘what am I supposed to do with this?’ ” (C2P04 clinical nurse consultant). Moreover, centres’ focus on cancer treatment meant that pain care struggled to gain traction even when a process could be instituted: “ unless pain is the presenting complaint and is at the forefront it goes into those, sorts of, you know, the ‘other details’ ” (C5P06 palliative care physician). For the palliative care centre, where pain care was already prioritised, there were doubts about how the proposed process improved on those already in place: “ I generally ask pretty detailed questions about pain anyway [so don’t need patients to be screened in the waiting room] ” (C6P04 pain medicine physician).

Suggestions for better integrating the intervention included “in-building” (C3P04 medical oncologist) responsibility for the strategies within new staff roles or introducing the strategies gradually by means of a “ multistep process” (C5P04 medical oncologist, head of department and clinical champion). Features of two strategies were singled out as having positive potential for supporting existing processes of care. The patient resource was said to “ facilitate communication between the oncology teams and the palliative care team ” (C5P05 radiation oncologist) and serve as a “ visual cue ” (C3P02 medical oncologist) to cover educational topics that “ they might have otherwise forgotten ” (C2P01 palliative care physician and clinical champion). Participants also found the spaced education email administration, spacing and repetition “ easy to manage ” (C2P01 palliative care physician and clinical champion) within their daily routines.

Promote patient-clinician partnership on pain care

Several participants expressed surprise at the prevalence of moderate-severe pain in screening results, and acknowledged that this revealed under-reporting of pain in usual care. Under-reporting was perceived to stem partly from patient expectations that pain from cancer was “ normal ” (C4P03 nurse practitioner) and to be especially common in the context of certain generational or cultural attitudes towards pain and opioids (“ I certainly think there’s a cultural element but there’s also your elderly patients who you know have been through the war and they’re just used to coping with things and you just suck it up … it’s like a badge of honour to be able to say ‘I’m not one of these pill-takers ’” (C3P03 registered nurse [RN])) or when patients were concerned that reporting pain might reduce their fitness for anti-cancer treatment: “[ patients might think that] if I tell them honestly how crappy I am with other symptoms and pain and everything, then they might stop my chemo” (C3P02 palliative care physician). Several participants perceived that under-reporting was also due to patients taking an overly passive role in consultations: “[clinicians assume that] if the patient doesn’t bring it up, it’s not a problem for them and … then the patient [is] thinking ‘the doctor will only talk about important things that are important for me and I won’t mention it because obviously it’s not important’ ” (C3P02 palliative care physician and clinical champion).

The screening component of the intervention was considered to address under-reporting by “ normal[ising] ” pain care, thus encouraging disclosure. The patient resource was also considered helpful for building patient capability to partner with clinicians on pain management by “ encouraging self-efficacy ” (C2P01 palliative care physician and clinical champion) through the tools it provided and its positive message that “ you can get control of your pain ” (C3P02 palliative care physician and clinical champion). It was also perceived to help patients “ keep a record ” (C5P03 clinical nurse consultant) of breakthrough pain and analgesia to discuss in their consultation. However, some participants delineated patient groups who might be less able to use the resource, including those with lower educational levels who struggled to set goals and identify an ‘acceptable’ level of pain balanced against side-effects from pharmacological management. For these patients, it was suggested that too many resources could be overwhelming rather than supportive: “ it’s almost like, the more resources they have, the less resourced there are ” (C5P06 RN). At one centre with an especially diverse demographic, patients were said to require substantial support even to understand the purpose and process of pain screening: “ most [patients] look at you going ‘oh, do I have to do anything?’ … They don’t want to read the [instruction] page which is relatively simple ” (C2P03 clinic receptionist).

Invest in clinical champions

All participants perceived the role of clinical champion to be pivotal to the intervention’s success. Champions were perceived to have two major responsibilities: advocating for the intervention among colleagues to boost motivation and providing practical support to build capability.

To be effective advocates, champions were perceived to need support from senior management ( “[leadership of change] it’s got to happen from the top ” (C5P02 RN)) as well as established, cordial relationships with colleagues they could leverage to motivate engagement: “ it also relies on the champion’s personal relationship with the staff which you’re asking to perform these roles and trying to change their management ” (C1P01 palliative care physician and clinical champion). Where champions felt under-supported by management, they relied on moral support from the project team to sustain their advocacy work: “ being the champion, and sometimes being the nagging champion, it actually felt quite nice to have the back-up of other people ” (C1P01 palliative care physician and clinical champion). Both physicians and nurses perceived the champion role might better suit the scope of practice of a junior doctor or senior nurse rather than consultants, based on their willingness to engage and approachability: “ realistically, you’re probably always going to get more engagement with registrars compared to consultants, unless it’s their own trial ” (C5P07 radiation oncologist); “ just give it [the role] to the CNCs [clinical nurse consultants] because as a general rule they’re the best at everything and have the best relationships with the patient ” (C3P04 medical oncologist).

From a practical perspective, clinical champions were expected to provide human resources for establishing and supporting pain screening and patient education: “ you need a body ” (C2P04 clinical nurse consultant). Unfortunately, however, champions across centres reported having limited time protected for the role within their usual duties: “ there just wasn’t the manpower to do that here ” (C3P02 palliative care physician and clinical champion). One suggestion for boosting capacity was to narrow the focus to one clinic and delegate practical tasks to less senior delegates than required for advocacy to render the time commitment more cost-effective: “[ it] might have been better to focus on one clinic and have full-time … junior nurse ” (C5P05 radiation oncologist). This presented an opportunity to train more than one clinical champion to provide better coverage across shifts and safeguard against the risk of losing champions to staff turnover.

Increasing pain awareness is the first step: Plan for slow incremental change rather than rapid uptake

While the barriers above meant only modest practice changes could be achieved, champions at half the centres perceived incremental progress had been made through increasing awareness among personnel regarding pain care as a focus for improvement: “ I think just trying to make pain something that people think about was probably one of the better strategies ” (C1P01 palliative care physician and clinical champion); it’s more at the top of our minds to remember, to screen the pain at every visit ” (C2P01 palliative care physician and clinical champion); “ I think it has highlighted those issues for us and we now need to take this on ” (C5P04 medical oncologist, head of department and clinical champion). Both nursing and medical participants at Centre 5 emphasized the need to be persistent in striving for continuous improvement: “ I think to get practice change, even for well-motivated people, I think it just needs to be pushed … they’ve done similar things with hand washing for doctors and it’s finally getting through ” (C504 medical oncologist and clinical champion); “ it would take more than just one of these kind of programs to get people to change ” (C5P03 clinical nurse consultant). Encouragingly, participants at this and one other centre expected some clinicians to continue using the patient education booklet and resource after the project ended: “ I’d just love to continue using these booklets ” (C5P02 RN); “[the] patient-held resource has been useful and has been taken up by people and I think they will continue to use those ” (C6P02 palliative care physician and clinical champion).

This qualitative sub-study of a cluster randomized controlled trial identified centre-level capabilities to be the most influential factors impeding or facilitating guideline implementation strategies for improving pain care for outpatients with cancer. Findings suggest that future initiatives of this kind should: consider centre readiness for change; articulate and deliver on the intervention’s value proposition; define clinician roles and responsibilities; integrate the intervention within existing systems and processes; promote patient partnership; invest in the clinical champion role, drawing from senior nurses and junior doctors, with support from medical leaders and management; and design the initiative around slow incremental change rather than rapid uptake.

Our findings are largely consistent with those from an ethnographic study exploring factors influencing implementation of cancer pain guidelines in Korean hospital cancer units, which identified a ‘lack of receptivity for change’ to be a key barrier [ 28 ]. However, observations from the Korean study suggested that a lack of centre leadership and cultural norms regarding nursing hierarchy were the most important underlying factors, whereas our Australian sample focused more on constraints imposed by centre systems and processes and a lack of clarity regarding disciplinary roles. These factors were consistently emphasized regardless of participants’ discipline and seniority, including by one centre’s head of department. Consistent with these findings, a recent Australian qualitative sub-study of anxiety/depression guideline implementation in oncology centres found greater role flexibility to be a key factor underpinning organisational readiness for change [ 29 ]. This team also provided quantitative evidence consistent with our finding that centres’ readiness for change is associated with personnel’s perception of benefit from guideline implementation [ 30 ]. Future initiatives should work harder to persuade clinicians of the intervention’s rationale and evidence base prior to commencement, given that perceptions of coherence and effectiveness are key dimensions of acceptability required for clinicians to invest time and effort [ 31 ]. Since our Trial was conducted, evidence has emerged for an impact from cancer symptom screening on survival that could be used persuasively [ 32 ]. Furthermore, the spaced education module might be more acceptable if made adjustable to the knowledge levels of a broader range of clinicians.

Other studies on implementation of cancer pain guidelines [ 11 , 13 ] suggest that structured approaches to process change tend to be more successful than less prescriptive approaches of the kind taken in the Stop Cancer PAIN Trial. We provided centres with guideline implementation strategies but no clear guidance on how to integrate these within existing contexts - i.e. implementation of the implementation, or ‘meta-implementation’. It was wrongly assumed that clinical champions could support integration with centre processes based on their knowledge of local context, but this turned out to be unreasonable given champions’ limited time for the role and lack of training in change management. Like most research to date [ 33 , 34 ], our trial focused largely on the advocacy role played by clinical champions, neglecting more practical and time consuming aspects that our interviews identified to be just as important. We join others in calling for more research on the mechanisms by which clinical champions can optimally facilitate change and ways to maximize their efficacy through training and support [ 24 ]. This should include exploration of optimal models by which different aspects of the champion role might be shared between more than one person where no-one is available with all the necessary attributes, as well as ways to ensure sustainability after support from the project team is withdrawn.

Theory-based research suggests that adding complex interventions to complex healthcare systems creates dynamic interplay and feedback loops, making consequences hard to predict [ 35 ]. In the current trial, this was likely exacerbated by our attempt to combine multiple strategies targeting patient, clinician and centre levels. We chose each strategy based on evidence for its stand-alone efficacy, and combined strategies rather than used them singly with the intent of leveraging complementary mechanisms, as recommended by the COM-B Model and US Institute of Medicine [ 36 ]. However, findings from our interviews suggest that interactions between the strategies and local processes separated their spheres of influence, precluding intended synergies. The Stop Cancer PAIN Trial is not alone in having over-estimated the value of combining guideline implementation strategies; a recent systematic review found that 8 other multi-component interventions similarly demonstrated limited effects on guideline adherence and patient outcomes [ 37 ]. Collectively, these findings suggest that future attempts at combining strategies should consider complex systems theory as well as behaviour change frameworks at each of a number of stages [ 38 ]. Alternatively, a more manageable approach for most cancer centres might be to focus on just one component at a time, periodically reviewing progress against SMART goals and, depending on results, supplementing with additional components using plan-do-study cycles [ 39 ].

Given the challenges with integrating screening into centre processes, it seems unlikely that improvements in pain scores during the control phase reported in our primary results article were due to the spontaneous use of screening data in consultations [ 18 ]. Indeed, while routine use of patient-reported outcome measures (PROMs) in oncology has been researched for more than a quarter-century [ 40 ], benefits to patient outcomes have only recently been demonstrated in the context of electronically-administered PROMs (ePROMs) that enable remote self-reporting, real-time feedback to clinicians, and clinician-patient telecommunication [ 12 ]. Further research is needed on how best to support clinician engagement with ePROMs, including training on how to use results in partnership with patients to assist shared decision-making and self-management [ 41 ].

A worrying finding from the current study was that some or all aspects of pain care were perceived to fall between the scopes of practice for oncology clinicians from each discipline. Clinical practice guidelines emphasize the need for pain care to be inter-disciplinary in recognition of the need for comprehensive assessment, non-pharmacological as well as pharmacological management, and patient education and support for self-management [ 42 ]. While the patient self-management resource included in the intervention was perceived to support communication between clinicians and patients, its potential for assisting coordination of care between disciplines was limited where roles and responsibilities were not previously established. Our findings and other research suggest that future initiatives may benefit from ‘process mapping’ with clinicians to identify where clinical workflow and roles might be reconfigured to incorporate the various aspects of pain care in the most efficient ways that do not substantially add to workload [ 41 ].

Patient education has been proven to improve pain outcomes by clinical trials [ 43 , 44 ], and we have argued previously that supporting pain self-management should be core business for all clinicians working in cancer care [ 45 ]. The ‘coaching’ approach needed to empower patients to recognize themselves as ‘experts’ on their pain and equal partners with clinicians in its management is iterative rather than a single event, and is ideally built on established and ongoing therapeutic relationships of trust with a particular team member. However, findings from patient education research more generally suggest that patient education and behaviour change is also optimally supported when key messages are reinforced by differing disciplinary perspectives [ 46 ]. Results from the current study suggest that these principles of pain care need more formal recognition within the scope of practice of oncology clinicians to ensure they are afforded sufficient time alongside anti-cancer treatment and related supportive care. Findings also indicate that clinicians may require training in the person-centred, partnership-oriented aspects of pain care beyond the educational approach used in the Stop Cancer PAIN Trial and other research [ 47 ]. Such training should be repeated regularly to ensure it reaches the majority of personnel at cancer centres, allowing for turnover.

Limitations

The current study had several limitations. Transferability even within Australia is limited by a focus on metropolitan services in only three out of eight jurisdictions. Data relied on clinician perspectives, and the response rate was less than one quarter of personnel at each centre, with the disciplines and specialties of participants being unrepresentative of centre workforces. Over-sampling of medical compared to nursing personnel likely reflects the fact that all clinical champions were medical consultants, while the predominance of palliative care physicians among medical participants presumably arises from the central focus this specialty has on pain care. Notably, our sample included no perspectives from allied health disciplines, despite the important roles these can play in non-pharmacological pain management. Confirmability was threatened by the potential for cognitive bias among researchers towards a favourable view of the intervention given their long-standing investment as members of the project team. We attempted to offset this by explicitly inviting criticism of the intervention from participants, and having the initial analysis conducted by researchers with no prior involvement in the project. A final limitation concerns reliance on the COM-B Model for analysis rather than an alternative framework or more inductive approach. While the COM-B has been widely used to explore barriers and facilitators across a wide range of healthcare interventions, we applied the model in a somewhat novel way to systems and processes as well as individuals’ behaviour after finding that participants perceived their agency to be majorly constrained by these. An implementation framework such as the integrated-Promoting Action on Research Implementation in Health Service (i-PARIHS) framework (iPARIHS) [ 48 ] or Consolidated Framework for Implementation Research (CFIR) [ 49 ] would have conceived of factors and their relationships in alternative ways that might have proven equally informative [ 50 ].

This qualitative sub-study elucidated important factors influencing the success of guideline implementation strategies at six cancer centres in the Stop Cancer PAIN Trial. Findings underscore the value that a qualitative approach offers for understanding the role of context when evaluating complex interventions [ 51 ]. Ultimately, the Stop Cancer PAIN Trial may have been overly ambitious in the scale of its intervention, especially given limited resources available at each centre. Further research is needed to understand how multi-component guideline implementation strategies can be optimally introduced within the context of local roles, systems and processes.

Availability of data and materials

The qualitative interview datasets generated and analysed during the current study are not publicly available due to the conditions of ethical approval which acknowledge the risk of participant re-identification.

Van Den Beuken-Van MH, Hochstenbach LMJ, Joosten EAJ, Tjan-Heijnen VCG, Janssen DJA. Update on prevalence of pain in patients with cancer: systematic review and meta-analysis. J Pain Symptom Manag. 2016;51(6):1070–90.

Article   Google Scholar  

Luckett T, Davidson PM, Green A, Boyle F, Stubbs J, Lovell M. Assessment and management of adult cancer pain: a systematic review and synthesis of recent qualitative studies aimed at developing insights for managing barriers and optimizing facilitators within a comprehensive framework of patient care. J Pain Symptom Manag. 2013;46(2):229–53.

Oldenmenger WH, Sillevis Smitt PAE, van Dooren S, Stoter G, van der Rijt CCD. A systematic review on barriers hindering adequate cancer pain management and interventions to reduce them: a critical appraisal. Eur J Cancer. 2009;45(8):1370–80.

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Jacobsen R, Sjogren P, Moldrup C, Christrup L. Physician-related barriers to cancer pain management with opioid analgesics: a systematic review. J Opioid Manag. 2007;3(4):207–14.

Jacobsen R, Moldrup C, Christrup L, Sjogren P. Patient-related barriers to cancer pain management: a systematic exploratory review. Scand J Caring Sci. 2009;23(1):190–208.

Jacobsen R, Liubarskiene Z, Moldrup C, Christrup L, Sjogren P, Samsanaviciene J. Barriers to cancer pain management: a review of empirical research. Medicina (Kaunas). 2009;45:427–33.

Fazeny B, Muhm M, Hauser I, Wenzel C, Mares P, Berzlanovich A, et al. Barriers in cancer pain management. Wiener Klinische Wochenschrift. 2000;112(22):978–81.

CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Makhlouf SM, Pini S, Ahmed S, Bennett MI. Managing pain in people with cancer—a systematic review of the attitudes and knowledge of professionals, patients, caregivers and Public. J Cancer Educ. 2020;35(2):214–40.

Roberto A, Greco MT, Uggeri S, Cavuto S, Deandrea S, Corli O, et al. Living systematic review to assess the analgesic undertreatment in cancer patients. Pain Pract. 2022;22(4):487–96.

Brink-Huis A, van Achterberg T, Schoonhoven L, Brink-Huis A, van Achterberg T, Schoonhoven L. Pain management: a review of organisation models with integrated processes for the management of pain in adult cancer patients. J Clin Nurs. 2008;17(15):1986–2000.

Du Pen SL, Du Pen AR, Polissar N, Hansberry J, Kraybill BM, Stillman M, et al. Implementing guidelines for cancer pain management: results of a randomized controlled clinical trial. J Clin Oncol. 1999;17(1):361–70.

Article   CAS   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Govindaraj R, Agar M, Currow D, Luckett T. Assessing patient-reported outcomes in routine cancer clinical care using electronic administration and telehealth technologies: Realist synthesis of potential mechanisms for improving health outcomes. J Med Internet Res. 2023;25:e48483.

Article   PubMed   PubMed Central   Google Scholar  

Koesel N, Tocchi C, Burke L, Yap T, Harrison A. Symptom distress: implementation of palliative care guidelines to improve pain, fatigue, and anxiety in patients with advanced cancer. Clin J Oncol Nurs. 2019;23(2):149–55.

PubMed   Google Scholar  

Dulko D, Hertz E, Julien J, Beck S, Mooney K. Implementation of cancer pain guidelines by acute care nurse practitioners using an audit and feedback strategy. J Am Acad Nurse Pract. 2010;22(1):45–55.

Samuelly-Leichtag G, Adler T, Eisenberg E. Something must be wrong with the implementation of cancer-pain treatment guidelines. A lesson from referrals to a pain clinic. Rambam Maimonides Med J. 2019;10(3):18.

Lopez Lopez R, Camps Herrero C, Khosravi-Shahi P, Guillem Porta V, Carrato Mena A, Garcia-Foncillas J, et al. Oncologist’s knowledge and implementation of guidelines for breakthrough cancer pain in Spain: CONOCE study. Clin Transl Oncol. 2018;20(5):613–8.

Luckett T, Phillips J, Agar M, Lam L, Davidson PM, McCaffrey N, et al. Protocol for a phase III pragmatic stepped wedge cluster randomised controlled trial comparing the effectiveness and cost-effectiveness of screening and guidelines with, versus without, implementation strategies for improving pain in adults with cancer attending outpatient oncology and palliative care services: the Stop Cancer PAIN trial. BMC Health Serv Res. 2018;18(1):1–13.

Lovell MR, Phillips JL, Luckett T, Lam L, Boyle FM, Davidson PM, et al. Effect of cancer pain guideline implementation on pain outcomes among adult outpatients with cancer-related pain: a stepped wedge cluster randomized trial. JAMA Netw Open. 2022;5(2):1–12.

Lovell M, Luckett T, Boyle F, Stubbs J, Phillips J, Davidson PM, et al. Adaptation of international guidelines on assessment and management of cancer pain for the Australian context. Asia-Pac J Clin Oncol. 2015;11(2):170–7.

Phillips JL, Heneka N, Bhattarai P, Fraser C, Shaw T. Effectiveness of the spaced education pedagogy for clinicians’ continuing professional development: a systematic review. Med Educ. 2019;53(9):886–902.

Cancer Council Australia. Overcoming cancer pain: A guide for people with cancer, their families and friends 2013 [Available from: http://www.cancercouncil.com.au/wp-content/uploads/2014/01/Can487-Overcoming-Pain-NSW-Lores2.pdf .

Luckett T, Davidson PM, Green A, Marie N, Birch MR, Stubbs J, et al. Development of a cancer pain self-management resource to address patient, provider, and health system barriers to care. Palliat Support Care. 2019;17(4):472–8.

Michie S, Atkins L, West R. The behaviour change wheel: a guide to designing interventions. London: Silverback Publishing; 2014.

Google Scholar  

Santos WJ, Graham ID, Lalonde M, Demery Varin M, Squires JE. The effectiveness of champions in implementing innovations in health care: a systematic review. Implement Sci Commun. 2022;3(1):80.

Ramanadhan S, Revette AC, Lee RM, Aveling EL. Pragmatic approaches to analyzing qualitative data for implementation science: an introduction. Implement Sci Commun. 2021;2(1):70.

Tong A, Sainsbury P, Craig J. Consolidated criteria for reporting qualitative research (COREQ): a 32-item checklist for interviews and focus groups. Int J Qual Health Care. 2007;19(6):349–57.

Gale NK, Heath G, Cameron E, Rashid S, Redwood S. Using the framework method for the analysis of qualitative data in multi-disciplinary health research. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2013;13(1):117.

Kim M, Jeong S, McMillan M, Higgins I. Translation of policy and guidelines into practice: lessons learned from implementation of the cancer pain management guideline. J Hosp Manage Health Policy. 2020;4.  https://doi.org/10.21037/jhmhp.2020.02.01 .

Geerligs L, Shepherd HL, Butow P, Shaw J, Masya L, Cuddy J, et al. What factors influence organisational readiness for change? Implementation of the Australian clinical pathway for the screening, assessment and management of anxiety and depression in adult cancer patients (ADAPT CP). Support Care Cancer. 2021;29(6):3235–44.

Faris MM, Shepherd HL, Butow PN, Kelly P, He S, Rankin N, et al. Staff- and service-level factors associated with organisational readiness to implement a clinical pathway for the identification, assessment, and management of anxiety and depression in adults with cancer. BMC Health Serv Res. 2023;23(1):866.

Sekhon M, Cartwright M, Francis JJ. Acceptability of healthcare interventions: an overview of reviews and development of a theoretical framework. BMC Health Serv Res. 2017;17(1):88.

Caminiti C, Maglietta G, Diodati F, Puntoni M, Marcomini B, Lazzarelli S, et al. The effects of patient-reported outcome screening on the survival of people with cancer: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Cancers. 2022;14(21):5470.

Morena AL, Gaias LM, Larkin C. Understanding the role of clinical champions and their impact on clinician behavior change: the need for causal pathway mechanisms. Front Health Serv. 2022;2:896885.

Miech EJ, Rattray NA, Flanagan ME, Damschroder L, Schmid AA, Damush TM. Inside help: an integrative review of champions in healthcare-related implementation. SAGE Open Med. 2018;6:2050312118773261.

Braithwaite J, Churruca K, Long JC, Ellis LA, Herkes J. When complexity science meets implementation science: a theoretical and empirical analysis of systems change. BMC Med. 2018;16:1–14.

Institute of Medicine (US) Committee on Standards for Developing Trustworthy Clinical Practice Guidelines. Clinical Practice Guidelines We Can Trust. In: Graham R, Mancher M, Miller Wolman D, Greenfield S, Steinberg E, editors. Washington, D.C.: National Academies Press; 2011.

Bora A-M, Piechotta V, Kreuzberger N, Monsef I, Wender A, Follmann M, et al. The effectiveness of clinical guideline implementation strategies in oncology—a systematic review. BMC Health Serv Res. 2023;23(1):347.

McGill E, Er V, Penney T, Egan M, White M, Meier P, et al. Evaluation of public health interventions from a complex systems perspective: a research methods review. Soc Sci Med. 2021;272: 113697.

Institute for Healthcare Improvement. Quality improvement essentials toolkit. 2017.

Trowbridge RDW, Jay SJ, et al. Determining the effectiveness of a clinical-practice intervention in improving the control of pain in outpatient with cancer. Acad Med. 1997;72:798–800.

Howell D, Rosberger Z, Mayer C, Faria R, Hamel M, Snider A, et al. Personalized symptom management: a quality improvement collaborative for implementation of patient reported outcomes (PROs) in ‘real-world’ oncology multisite practices. J Patient Rep Outcomes. 2020;4(1):47.

Australian Adult Cancer Pain Management Guideline Working Party. Autralian clinical pathway for screening, assessment and management of cancer pain in adults Sydney, Australia: Cancer Council Australia. 2013. http://wiki.cancer.org.au/australia/Guidelines:Cancer_pain_management/Flowchart_overview .

Marie N, Luckett T, Davidson P, Lovell M, Lal S. Optimal patient education for cancer pain: a systematic review and theory-based meta-analysis. Support Care Cancer. 2013;21:3529–37.

Abernethy AP, Currow DC, Shelby-James T, Rowett D, May F, Samsa GP, et al. Delivery strategies to optimize resource utilization and performance status for patients with advanced life-limiting illness: results from the palliative care trial [ISRCTN 81117481]. J Pain Symptom Manage. 2013;45(3):488–505.

Lovell MR, Luckett T, Boyle FM, Phillips J, Agar M, Davidson PM. Patient education, coaching, and self-management for cancer pain. J Clin Oncol. 2014;32(16):1712–20.

Luckett T, Roberts MM, Smith T, Swami V, Cho JG, Wheatley JR. Patient perspectives on how to optimise benefits from a breathlessness service for people with COPD. NPJ Prim Care Respir Med. 2020;30(1). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41533-020-0172-4 .

Phillips JL, Heneka N, Lovell M, Lam L, Davidson P, Boyle F, et al. A phase III wait-listed randomised controlled trial of novel targeted inter-professional clinical education intervention to improve cancer patients’ reported pain outcomes (the Cancer Pain Assessment (CPAS) Trial): study protocol. Trials. 2019;20:1–12.

Harvey G, Kitson A. PARIHS revisited: from heuristic to integrated framework for the successful implementation of knowledge into practice. Implement Sci. 2016;11(1):33.

Damschroder LJ, Reardon CM, Widerquist MAO, Lowery J. The updated consolidated framework for implementation research based on user feedback. Implement Sci. 2022;17(1):1–16.

Geerligs L, Rankin NM, Shepherd HL, Butow P. Hospital-based interventions: a systematic review of staff-reported barriers and facilitators to implementation processes. Implement Sci. 2018;13(1):36.

Skivington K, Matthews L, Simpson SA, Craig P, Baird J, Blazeby JM, et al. A new framework for developing and evaluating complex interventions: update of medical research council guidance. BMJ. 2021;374:n2061.

Download references

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to dedicate this article to the memory of Sally Fielding, who worked as a valued member of the project team throughout the Stop Cancer PAIN Trial. We would also like to acknowledge the contributions of project manager A/Prof Annmarie Hosie, data manager Dr Seong Cheah, and research assistant Layla Edwards.

This research was supported by a grant from the National Breast Cancer Foundation.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

IMPACCT Centre—Improving Palliative, Aged and Chronic Care through Clinical Research and Translation, Faculty of Health, University of Technology Sydney (UTS), Building 10, 235 Jones St, Ultimo, Sydney, NSW, 2007, Australia

Tim Luckett, Meera Agar & Maja Garcia

School of Nursing and Centre for Healthcare Transformation, Queensland University of Technology (QUT), Brisbane, QLD, Australia

Jane Phillips

South West Sydney School of Clinical Medicine, University of New South Wales (UNSW), Sydney, NSW, Australia

The Limbic, Sydney, Australia

Linda Richards

Palliative Care Department, Greenwich Hospital, HammondCare, Sydney, NSW, Australia

Najwa Reynolds & Melanie Lovell

University of Wollongong, Wollongong, NSW, Australia

Patricia Davidson & David Currow

Charles Perkins Centre, School of Medical Sciences, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia

Patricia Ritchie Centre for Cancer Care and Research, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia

Frances Boyle

Northern Medical School, The University of Sydney, Sydney, NSW, Australia

Frances Boyle & Melanie Lovell

Macau University of Science and Technology, Macau, China

Lawrence Lam

Deakin Health Economics, Institute for Health Transformation, School of Health and Social Development, Deakin University, Melbourne, Australia

Nikki McCaffrey

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Contributions

TL, JP, MA, PMD, TS, DCC, FB, LL, NM and ML contributed to the concept and design of this research. TL, LR, MR, MG and ML contributed to the acquisition, analysis or interpretation of the data. TL and ML contributed to drafting of the manuscript. All authors contributed to revisions of the manuscript and approved the final version.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Tim Luckett .

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate.

This research was approved by the Southwestern Sydney Local Health District Human Research Ethics Committee (HREC/14/LPOOL/479). All participants gave written informed consent to participate.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/ .

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Luckett, T., Phillips, J., Agar, M. et al. Factors influencing fidelity to guideline implementation strategies for improving pain care at cancer centres: a qualitative sub-study of the Stop Cancer PAIN Trial. BMC Health Serv Res 24 , 969 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12913-024-11243-1

Download citation

Received : 10 April 2024

Accepted : 25 June 2024

Published : 22 August 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1186/s12913-024-11243-1

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Implementation
  • Qualitative

BMC Health Services Research

ISSN: 1472-6963

what is motivation in a research paper

COMMENTS

  1. What is the difference between "introduction" and "motivation" sections

    Motivation: WHY. The motivation section explains the importance behind your research. Why should the reader care? Why is your research the most groundbreaking piece of scientific knowledge since sliced bread (or CRISPR/Cas9 genome editing)? Some people do combine the introduction and the motivation, it really depends on the research and the ...

  2. Motivation for Research and Publication: Experience as a Researcher and

    Research Motivation • Manage time and work systematically. For example, in time management, a systematic timetable will make life more manageable. Software such as Google Calendar can be used for this purpose. • Researchers must keep in mind that the main motivation in developing their research is their deep interest in the field ...

  3. PDF 1. What is motivation and why does it matter?

    These papers highlight findings from research and lessons from programs around the country that we felt could be useful to policymakers, educators, and others interested in improving student motivation. These papers are not intended to be a comprehensive summary of research or lessons learned, but rather an opening of a

  4. Full article: Motivation

    Münchow and Bannert (Citation 2019) pick up a theme that has been predominant in European research, that is, the importance of emotions in learning and motivation. Emotions research has, more recently, been impacting North America and international research (see, for example, Crocker et al., Citation 2013). The Münchow and Bannert study ...

  5. The Importance of Students' Motivation for Their Academic Achievement

    This is an important question with respect to motivation theory and future research in this field. Moreover, based on the findings it might be possible to better judge which kind of motivation should especially be fostered in school to improve achievement. This is important information for interventions aiming at enhancing students ...

  6. What is your motivation for writing a research paper?

    The conviction that ideas matter and that an individual researcher, a network of researchers and the academic community as a whole can - and should (try to) - make a difference. Forschen und ...

  7. The Behavioral Neuroscience of Motivation: An Overview of Concepts

    1 Why Motivation Is Important to Understand. Understanding what drives motivated behavior in humans is a truly fascinating endeavor. But as important as our curiosity for knowing what drives us as individuals, and what supports individual differences in levels of motivation among our friends and colleagues, is the critical question; why do motivational processes get disrupted when the clinical ...

  8. Teaching and researching motivation

    As a classic research topic in psychology, the study of motivation focuses on answering questions like why people act, in which direction, and with what intensity (Keller et al., 2019). Motivation ...

  9. What Is Motivation, Where Does It Come from, and How Does It Work

    The aim of motivation is therefore to bring about a desired (need, goal) state. Motivation underlies and organizes all aspects of a person's psychology. As it does so, motivation "glues" a person together as a functioning individual in their culture and context.

  10. Motivation Science

    Motivation Science is a multi-disciplinary journal that publishes significant contributions to the study of motivation, broadly conceived.. The journal publishes papers on diverse aspects of, and approaches to, the science of motivation, including work carried out in all subfields of psychology, cognitive science, economics, sociology, management science, organizational science, neuroscience ...

  11. Motivation: Introduction to the Theory, Concepts, and Research

    Motivation is a psychological construct that refers to the disposition to act and direct behavior according to a goal. Like most of psychological processes, motivation develops throughout the life span and is influenced by both biological and environmental factors. The aim of this chapter is to summarize research on the development of ...

  12. "What motivates me?" Motivation to conduct research of academics in

    Research activities of academics is a typical category of motivated behaviors, but scarce studies tested motivational regulations of research activities and subscales of research motivation. This status quo inspires the authors to capture the picture of research motivations. 2.2.

  13. How to explain things in the motivation section of a mathematical paper

    This is actually really hard and usually takes much more experience than it did to solve the research problem in the first place. So I think its common for e.g. a graduating PhD student to have the technical knowledge to solve the problem but to find it difficult to articulate where the problem lies within a much bigger field of inquiry.

  14. Research Proposal: Motivation and Background

    The first three sections of your research proposal make up the motivation for your research. These sections are: 1. Introduction. 2. Background. 3. Research Questions or Goals. Each of these sections should come under their own heading in your proposal, and you should fill out each of the sections as follows:

  15. Contribution vs Motivation

    Contribution: what is the new thing (result, approach, technique, whatever) that this paper describes, that advances the state of this field? For example: This paper describes a way to do X using Y, which is up to 200 times faster than the current state of the art.

  16. (PDF) Motivational Theories

    This paper explores theories of motivation, including instinct theory, arousal theory, incentive theory, intrinsic theory, extrinsic theory, the ARCS model, self-determination theory, expectancy ...

  17. Theories of motivation: A comprehensive analysis of human behavior

    This paper explores theories of motivation, including instinct theory, arousal theory, incentive theory, intrinsic theory, extrinsic theory, the ARCS model, self-determination theory, expectancy-value theory, and goal-orientation theory. Each theory is described in detail, along with its key concepts, assumptions, and implications for behavior.

  18. Motivation Research

    Motivation research is a term used to refer to a selection of qualitative research methods designed to probe consumers' minds to discover the deep, often subconscious or latent reasons and goals underlying everyday consumption and purchasing behaviors. Motivation research was the premier consumer research method used in the 1950s, leading to ...

  19. How to Write the Rationale of the Study in Research (Examples)

    The rationale of the study is the justification for taking on a given study. It explains the reason the study was conducted or should be conducted. This means the study rationale should explain to the reader or examiner why the study is/was necessary. It is also sometimes called the "purpose" or "justification" of a study.

  20. Motivational Research: Techniques, Strengths, and Weaknesses

    Motivational research is a type of marketing research that attempts to understand why customers act the way they do. Find more about it here. ... The researcher may administer many tests using pencil and paper under this method of motivation study to elicit replies from the participants regarding their wants, desires, opinions, interests ...

  21. MOTIVATION: CONCEPT, THEORIES AND PRACTICAL IMPLICATIONS

    Management researchers have long believed that organisational goals are unattainable without effective use of motivational concepts. One key element of leadership is the ability to get others to do something, creating an influence. This paper is designed to help familiarise readers with the concepts related to motivation.

  22. Motivation Research: Definition and Techniques

    Motivation research is a form of consumer research which has gained ground over the recent years. Motivation Research is the currently popular term used to describe the application of psychiatric and psychological techniques to obtain a better understanding of why people respond as they do to products, ads and various other marketing situations.

  23. Different motivation, different achievements: the relationship of

    Research on motivation and dedication to academic pursuits Several academic models and theories are dedicated to examining the motivation and dedication that individuals exhibit toward their ...

  24. (PDF) Motivation Research

    Motivation research is a term used to refer to a selection of qualitative research methods that were designed to probe consumers' minds in order to discover the subconscious or latent reasons and ...

  25. (PDF) A literature review on motivation

    Ó International Network of Business and Management 2013. Abstract Research on motivation has attracted academic and corporate entities. over the last two decades. In the present study, authors ...

  26. Design and Effect of Neurosurgical Educational Software Using

    Motivation Questionnaire: The content validity (CVR and CVI indices) of this questionnaire was confirmed by 10 experts in surgical technology, medical education, and e-learning. The value of the content validity ratio was in the range of 0.8 to 1 for the questionnaire items. ... The research sample was limited to 6th and 8th-semester surgical ...

  27. Factors influencing fidelity to guideline implementation strategies for

    Design. The intervention, methods and results of the Stop Cancer PAIN trial have been described in previous open-access articles [17, 18].The sub-study used a qualitative approach with pragmatic orientation to enable in-depth exploration of factors influencing success from the perspectives of clinicians at each participating centre [].Clinician views canvassed at interview were considered the ...